• Sonuç bulunamadı

Effects of soccer on stock markets: the return-volatility relationship

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Effects of soccer on stock markets: the return-volatility relationship"

Copied!
7
0
0

Yükleniyor.... (view fulltext now)

Tam metin

(1)

ContentslistsavailableatSciVerseScienceDirect

The

Social

Science

Journal

jo u rn al h om epa g e :w w w . e l s e v i e r . c o m / l o c a t e/ s o s c i j

Effects

of

soccer

on

stock

markets:

The

return–volatility

relationship

夽,夽夽

M.

Hakan

Berument

a,∗

,

Nildag

Basak

Ceylan

b

aDepartmentofEconomics,BilkentUniversity,06800,Ankara,Turkey

bDepartmentofBankingandFinance,YildirimBeyazitUniversity,06040,Ankara,Turkey

a

r

t

i

c

l

e

i

n

f

o

Articlehistory: Received25March2011

Receivedinrevisedform11March2012 Accepted12March2012

Availableonline16September2012

JELclassification: A12 C22 G12 L83

a

b

s

t

r

a

c

t

Thispaperassessestheeffectsofdomesticsoccerteams’performancesagainstforeign rivalsonstockmarketreturnsaswellasonthereturn–volatilityrelationship.Datafrom Chile,Spain,TurkeyandtheUnitedKingdomsupportpropositionsthatsoccerteamsresults ininternationalcupsaffectstockmarketreturnsandthereturn–volatilityrelationship. EvidencefromSpainandtheUK,soccerpowerhouses,suggeststhatlossesareassociated withlowerreturnsandhigherriskaversionbutevidencefromChileandTurkey,where socceristhemostimportantsportbutteamsarenotassuccessful,revealsthatwinsare associatedwithhigherreturnsandlowerriskaversion.

CrownCopyright©2012PublishedbyElsevierInc.onbehalfofWesternSocialScience Association.Allrightsreserved.

1. Introduction

Afundamentalassumptionofeconomicsisthatagents

arerationalindecisionmaking.Recentliterature,however,

haschallengedthisassumption.Stracca(2004)reviewsa

largebody ofevidence andfinds examplesof irrational

behaviorinnumerousstudies.Behavioraleconomicsand

behavioralfinancetrytoexplainhowemotionsand

cogni-tiveerrorsinfluenceinvestorsandtheirdecision-making

processes. Researchers can explain various stock

mar-ketanomalies,bubblesandcrashesusingpsychologyand

otherrelevantsocialsciencesmethods.Asthepsychology

literature suggests, “mood” is one of the sources of

夽 AlthoughthesportiscalledsoccerinNorthAmerica,itiscalled foot-ballintherestoftheworld.

夽夽 WewouldliketothankthestaffoftheEmbassyoftheTurkish RepublicinSantiago,ConsulateofChileinAnkara,IbrahimKirkayakof theTurkishRadioandTelevisionsportsservice,anonymousreferee,Rana NelsonandOnurIncefortheircontributions.

∗ Correspondingauthor.Tel.:+903122902342;fax:+903122665140. E-mail addresses: berument@bilkent.edu.tr (M.H. Berument),

nbceylan@ybu.edu.tr(N.B.Ceylan).

URL:http://www.bilkent.edu.tr/berument(M.H.Berument).

agent irrationality.FollowingEdmans, Garcia, and Norli

(2007)and Berument,Ceylan, andGozpinar (2006),this

paper claims that mood changes stemming from the

results of soccer matches affect stock market returns.

Thepresentstudyfurtherclaimsthatmoodchangesalso

affect the return–volatility relationship: agents become

morerisk averseafteralossand lessriskaverseaftera

win.

Inthisstudy,wetestwhethermoodhasasignificant

effectonassetprices.Toaccomplishthis,wefirstfindan

eventthatcanaffectthousandsofpeople’smoods

simul-taneously.Sportseventsprovidesuchanexample;soccer

matches in particular. The psychology literature

docu-mentsthatsoccerresultshave amuchlargerimpacton

mood than supposed bymost economistsbecausethey

affectmillionsofpeopleinasimilarway.Somestudies

dis-cusstheeffectof sportsresultsand mood.Forinstance,

Wann,Dolan,Mcgeorge,andAllison(1994)documentthat

fansoftenexperiencestrongpositivereactionsto

watch-ingtheirteamperformwell.Hirt,Erickson,Kennedy,and

Zillmann(1992)findthatIndianaUniversitycollege

stu-dentsestimatetheirownperformancestobesignificantly

betterafterwatchingawinbytheircollegebasketballteam

thanafterwatchingaloss.

0362-3319/$–seefrontmatter.CrownCopyright©2012PublishedbyElsevierInc.onbehalfofWesternSocialScienceAssociation.Allrightsreserved. doi:10.1016/j.soscij.2012.03.003

(2)

Mainstreameconomicssupportstheeffectofsocceron

economicperformance.Pollard(2002)documentsthe

rela-tionshipbetweenthegrowthrateofEuropeancountries

andWorldCupsuccess.Moreover,Berument,Inamlik,and

Yucel(2003)andBerumentandYucel(2005)findthat

suc-cessinsocceraffectsindustrialproductioninTurkey.

Similar to the present paper, Edmans et al. (2007),

Berument andYucel (2005) andBerument etal. (2006)

examinetheeffectsofinternationalsoccermatchesona

setofmacroeconomicvariablesviamood.Edmansetal.

(2007)investigatestockmarketreactionstotheoutcome

oftheinternationalsoccermatchesof42countries, and

findthatlossesin internationalsoccermatches havean

economicallyandstatisticallysignificantnegativeeffecton

thatcountry’sstockmarket.However,thesamestudydoes

notfindacorrespondingeffectafterwins:returnsondays

followingwinsareclosetozeroandnotstatistically

sig-nificant.Edmans etal.(2007)suggest thata soccerloss

effectiscausedbyachangeininvestormood.ForTurkey,

Berumentetal.(2006)examinetheeffectsofBesiktas’

suc-cessonstockreturnsandshowthatthisteam’swinsover

foreignrivalsincreasestockmarketreturns.Edmansetal.

(2007)andBerumentetal.(2006)considertheeffectof

lossesandwinsonlyonstockmarketreturns.Incontrast,

ourstudyconsidersnotonlythelevelofreturnbutalsothe

pricingofriskchangeswithsoccerscores.

The existing literature on behavioral finance mostly

concentratesonreturns.However,agents’riskperceptions,

whichthereturn–volatilityrelationshipmaycapture,may

alsochangewithmood.Loewenstein(2000),Romer(2000)

and Loewenstein, Weber, Hsee, and Welch(2001) note

thattheprobabilityweightingfunctionmaydependonan

agent’semotionalstate.Leahy(2002)modelsthe

invest-mentbehaviorofadepressedindividual.Accordingtothat

model,peoplewhoaredepressedbelievethattheyhave

fewpresentandfutureresourcesandbelievetheyhavea

lowutilityofgaininamarketthatisvolatileand

declin-ing.Therefore, depressedindividuals adoptstrategiesto

minimizetheirlosses.JohnsonandTversky(1983)indicate

thatmoodinfluencesjudgmentsofoutcomeprobability.

In one experiment, theyfindthat agents are less likely

togambleafteralossandmorelikelytogambleaftera

gain.Arkes,Herren,andIsen(1988)demonstratethatthe

salesof Ohio’sstate lottery ticketsincrease in thedays

afteravictorybytheOhioStateUniversitysoccerteam.

Similarly,Dohmenetal.(2006)reporttheresultsofa

tele-phone surveyconducted inGermany justone dayafter

theGermansoccerteam’sunexpectedgoodperformance

duringtheFIFA2006WorldCup.Theinterviewswere

re-performedontheafternoonofthenextday.Theauthors

notethatcurrentperceptionandfutureeconomic

expec-tationsimprovedatthepersonalandeconomy-widelevels.

HankeandKirchler(2010)arguethatastheimportance

of amatch increasesand itsresultis moreunexpected,

itsimpactwillbehigher.Edmansetal.(2007)alsonote

thatunderobjectiveprobabilities,marketdeclineisstrong

forunexpectedlosses.Thus,theeffectismorepronounced

afteralossforateamthatismorelikelytowin,i.e.,a

usu-allysuccessfulteam.Theeffectisalsomorepronounced

aftera winfora teamthat isless likelytowin.Tothis

end,wedocumentresultsfromfourcountrieswherethe

soccer is the most important sport. Teams from Spain

and the UK are considered to bemore successfulthan

teamsfromChileandTurkey.Weexpecttoseethatlosses

havelargernegativeeffectsforSpainandtheUKandwe

expectthatwinshavelargerpositiveeffectsforChileand

Turkey.

In this study, we examine the effects of the results

ofinternationalsoccermatchesagainstforeignrivalson

stock exchanges and we report the effect of wins and

lossesoffourcountries’importantsoccerteamsonstock

returns,along withtheireffects onrisk perception.We

organizetherestofthepaperasfollows:Section2

elab-oratesonthemodelthatweuseandSection3provides

the empirical evidence. The last section concludes the

paper.

2. Methodology

In order to assess the effects of soccer matches on

stockmarketreturnsandreturn–volatilityrelationships,

weuseaclassofAutoregressiveConditional

Heteroskedas-tic(ARCH)specifications.TheARCHspecificationcaptures

theconditionalvarianceofstockreturnsasameasureof

risk.

Inthispaper,weuseNelson’s(1991)EGARCH

specifica-tionforconditionalmodelingbecauseofvariousappealing

features.First,itremovespartofthenon-negativity

param-eter restrictions of the conditional variance within the

traditional Bollerslev’s (1986) GARCH model. Second, it

allowsstockmarketshockstohaveasymmetriceffectson

conditionalvariance.Thatis,positiveandnegativeshocks

may affect risk perception differently, i.e., the leverage

effect.

Here,wewanttoseetheeffectsofmoodonrealstock

returns and return–volatility relationships. Our model

specificationis: Rt=xtˇ+ht+·outcomet+ϕ·outcomet·ht+εt (1) εt∼(0,h2t) logh2 t =+ p



i=1 ıilogh2t−i+ q



j=1 j





εt−j ht−j





−E





εt−j ht−j





+εt−j ht−j



(2)

whereRt isthestockreturncalculatedbytakingthe

log-arithmicfirstdifferenceofthedailyclosingpriceofstock

marketsandx

tisavectorforexogenousvariablesattime

t.ThemodelincludesdailydummiesforMonday,Tuesday,

Wednesday,ThursdayandFridaytoaccountforthe

day-of-the-weekeffect.Thexvectoralsoincludeslaggedvaluesof

thereturnbecauseCosimanoandJansen(1988)arguethat

iferrorsareautocorrelated,thenEngle’s(1982)ARCH-LM

testsuggeststhepresenceoftheARCHeffectevenwhenit

isnotpresent.Theconditionalstandard deviationofthe

residualterm,ht,is taken asa proxyfor risk attime t;

outcometisthedummyvariableforthewinsorlossesand

takesthevalueofonefor thenextbusinessdayaftera

winoralossininternationalmatches,andzerootherwise.1

1 Wenotethatwithinthesampleperiodtherewerenevertwoormore

wins(orlosses)tobeconsideredonthenextbusinessday.Therefore, outcometnevertakesthevalueoftwoorabove.

(3)

Outcomet·htistheinteractivetermforwinsorloseswith

theconditionalstandarddeviationoftheresidualterm.The

interactivetermisintendedtocapturethechangeinthe

return–volatilityrelationshipwithwinsorlossesandεtis

theerrortermattimet.Asweexpectapositive

relation-shipbetweenwinsandreturns,wealsoexpectanegative

relationshipbetweenlossesandreturns.Thisexpectation

impliesthatthesignoftheestimatedoutcometcoefficient,

,willbepositiveforthewinsofChileandTurkeyand

neg-ativeforthelossesofSpainandtheUK.Ontheotherhand,

thetheoryonchangeinriskaversionsuggeststhatthesign

ofthecoefficientoftheinteractivetermbetweenht and

outcomet,ϕ,willbenegativeforChileandTurkeyand

pos-itiveforSpainandtheUK.Thispredictionisbecausewins

makepeoplelessriskaverse,ormoreriskloving,andlosses

makepeoplemoreriskaverse,orlessriskloving;thuswe

expecttoacceptthesamereturnwithahigherriskora

lowerreturnandalowerriskorahigherreturnwiththe

samerisklevel.

In Eq. (2), the parameter captures residual

asym-metric effects. When =0, a negative surprise, that is,

(εt−1/ht−1)<0, has the same effect on volatility as a

positivesurpriseof equalmagnitude.When−1<<0,a

negativesurpriseincreasesvolatilitymorethanapositive

surprisedoes.When<−1,apositivesurprisedecreases

volatilityandanegativesurpriseincreasesvolatility.

Teamsgenerallyplay alltheirmatchesafterbusiness

hours.MostmatchesareonTuesdays,Wednesdaysand

Thursdays.Therefore,thedummyvariable forwins and

losses,outcomet, forthe nextbusinessdaycan capture

higherreturnsassociatedwiththesedays.However,itis

importanttorecognizedummyvariablesforthe

day-of-the-weekeffect;thus,estimatesforwinsandlossescapture

theeffects ofwinsand lossesaftertheday-of-the-week

effectisaccountedfor.

3. Empiricalevidence

Toassesstheeffectof internationalsoccer scoreson

the stock market, we gather data from four countries

where soccer is an important sports activity;

interna-tionalordomesticsuccessesbelongtoafew teamsthat

havesupportersalloverthecountry.Mostpeople

asso-ciatethemselveswithoneofthefewteams,whichfurther

ensuresthatthesoccerresultswillhaveacountry-wide

effect.Forthesereasons,wechooseChile,Spain,Turkey

and theUK. We select Cobreloa, Colo Colo, Universitad

CatolicaandUniversitaddeChilesoccerteamsforChile;

BarcelonaandRealMadridsoccerteamsforSpain;

Besik-tas,FenerbahceandGalatasaraysoccerteamsforTurkey

and Arsenal, Chelsea, Liverpool and Manchester United

soccer teams for the UK. The time span of the data is

from 01 January 1985 to 02 February 2007, but may

varyamongcountries. Wegatherthedataforthestock

exchangesfromDatastream.Wecomputethestock

mar-ketreturnsbytakingthepercentagechangeofthedaily

closingpriceoftheGeneralStockPriceIndex(IGPA)for

Chile,theMadridStockExchangeGeneralIndexforSpain,

theIstanbulStockExchange100IndexforTurkey,andthe

FTSE100Index fortheUK.We obtaintheinternational

soccermatchresultsforthesefourcountriesmainlyfrom

http://www.rsssf.com.2

Table 1 reports the time period, teams, and

num-ber of international wins, losses, and ties, along with

theirpercentagestototalmatches.Table1suggeststhat

in terms of results, Spain and the UK have more

suc-cessful soccer teams than Chile and Turkey. Because

of the differences in results, we might observe

differ-enteffects oflossesand winsbetweenthetwocountry

sets.

Table2reportsdescriptivestatisticsforthestock

mar-ketreturnsofeachcountry.Notethatexcesskurtosis,the

kurtosisvalueisgreaterthanthree, andthestatistically

significantJarque-BeraTestsfornormalitysuggest

time-varyingvariancesforthereturns.

Table3reportstheestimatesofsoccermatchoutcomest

on stock market returns and return–volatility

relation-ships for the four countries. The estimated coefficients

of outcomet and outcomet·ht are important; however,

we discuss other coefficients to show that our

find-ings parallel the existing literature on the respective

models.

First, the Mondayeffect is negative for allcountries

but Spain,and Tuesday has a negative and statistically

significant effect for Spain.Wednesday alsohas a

neg-ative and statistically significant effect for Spain only.

Thursdays’coefficientsarepositiveforallbutSpain.For

Chile,theThursdayeffectispositiveandstatistically

sig-nificant. Friday effects are positive and are statistically

significantfor Chileat 1%,for Turkeyat5% and forthe

UKat10%significancelevels.Therefore,Mondayreturns

arelowerthanFridayreturnsforallcountriesbutSpain.

However,FridayreturnsarehigherthanTuesdayreturns

for Spain. Thus, our findings are parallel to the

find-ingsintheliteratureontheday-of-the-weekorweekend

effects.

Second,therisktermcoefficient,ht,ispositiveforall

countries. The effectis statisticallysignificantat the1%

significance levelfor Spain,and fortheUKit is

statisti-callysignificantatthe10%significancelevel.Thepositive

coefficientssuggestthatriskincreasesexpectedstock

mar-ket returns.Thisfindingis inlinewiththemainstream

asset-pricing model, which indicates the association of

higherreturnswithriskierportfolios.

Third,consistentwiththeexistingliterature,outcomet

coefficientsarenegativewhentheSpanishandBritish

soc-certeamsloseandpositivewhentheChileanandTurkish

soccer teamswin. Thisfindingsupports theproposition

thathighermoraleincreasesstockmarketreturnsforthe

cases of wins and decreasesreturns after a loss. These

resultsmakesensebecauseSpainandtheUKare

success-fulcountriesinsoccer.Thus,winsmaynotaffectthemood

ofsoccerfansasmuchaslosses.Forthisreason,weexpect

theeffectsoflossestobehigherforthosecountries.Onthe

otherhand,ChileandTurkeyarenotassuccessfulinsoccer

asSpainandtheUK.Thus,winsaremorelikelythanlosses

2Wealsousehttp://www.statto.com/,http://kassiesa.home.xs4all.nl/ bert/uefa/data/index.html and http://www.eurocupshistory.com/uefa cup/tofindorvalidatesomeofthematchresults.

(4)

Table1

Thenumberofwins,tiesandlossesforeachcountry.

Sampleperiod Numberscoreson

Wins Ties Losses

Chile 02January1987–31December2006 94 73 90

(37%) (28%) (35%) Cobreloa 22 18 26 (33%) (27%) (40%) ColoColo 31 22 25 (40%) (28%) (32%) UniversitadCatolica 30 24 25 (38%) (30%) (32%) UniversitaddeChile 11 9 14

Spain 01November1977–02February2007 298 112 127

(55%) (21%) (24%)

Barcelona 151 62 60

(55%) (23%) (22%)

RealMadrid 147 50 67

(56%) (19%) (25%)

Turkey 03July1987–02February2007 99 65 109

(36%) (24%) (40%) Besiktas 27 19 30 (36%) (25%) (39%) Fenerbahce 24 11 35 (34%) (16%) (50%) Galatasaray 48 35 44 (38%) (28%) (34%)

UnitedKingdom 01January1985–02February2007 249 120 107

(52%) (25%) (23%) Arsenal 59 33 31 (48%) (27%) (25%) Chelsea 47 17 20 (56%) (20%) (24%) Liverpool 63 29 25 (54%) (25%) (21%) ManchesterUnited 80 41 31 (53%) (27%) (20%)

Source:Thedataonthesoccerscoresarefromhttp://www.rsssf.com.

Numbersinparenthesesarethepercentagesofthecorrespondingoutcomes(wins,tiesorlosses)tototalmatches.

toaffectthestockmarketreturnandthereturn–volatility

relationship.

Finally,weconsidertheinteractiveterm,outcomet·ht.

ThecoefficientsarenegativeforChileandTurkeyand

posi-tiveforSpainandtheUK.ThenegativecoefficientsforChile

andTurkeysuggestthatmarketscompensateagentsless

afterawinforbearingthesamelevelofrisk;thatis,agents

aremorerisklovingafterawin.Thepositivecoefficientsfor

SpainandtheUKsuggestthatmarketscompensateagents

moreafteralossforbearingthesamelevelofrisk;agents

becomemoreriskaverse.Weestimatethesame

specifica-tionsforSpain’sandtheUK’swinsandChile’sandTurkey’s

losses.Wedonotreporttheseestimatesheretosavespace

buttheyareavailablefromtheauthorsuponrequest.

Nei-therthe outcome coefficients northe interactiveterms

are statistically significant, supporting the proposition

Table2

Descriptivestatisticsofthecountries’stockmarketreturnsintherespectivesampleperiods.

Chile Spain Turkey UnitedKingdom

Mean 0.075580 0.050450 0.217808 0.032523

Standarddeviation 0.842854 1.057097 2.987632 1.006494

Skewness −0.258825 −0.118161 0.180407 −0.586982

Kurtosis 11.659901 5.622417 3.028642 9.673965

Jarque-Bera 29457.843566 10020.099049 1837.702786 22676.560464

Source:DataforthestockexchangesarefromDatastream.

Notes:Returnsarecalculatedbytakingthepercentagechangeofthedailyclosingpriceofthestockexchangeindexes.WeusedtheGeneralStockPrice Index(IGPA)forChile,theMadridStockExchangeGeneralIndexforSpain,theIstanbulStockExchange100IndexforTurkey,andtheFinancialTimesStock Exchange100IndexfortheUK.

(5)

Table3

Effectsofsoccermatchoutcomesonstockreturnsandthereturn–volatilityrelationship.

Outcome– losses Outcome–wins

Spain UK Chile Turkey

PanelA:estimatesforthereturnequation

Monday 0.0390* −0.0145 −0.1268*** −0.1728 (0.0761) (0.6509) (0.0000) (0.1122) Tuesday −0.0814*** 0.0303 0.0092 −0.1428 (0.0000) (0.4433) (0.7355) (0.1997) Wednesday −0.0485* 0.0425 0.0290 0.0744 (0.0643) (0.1824) (0.2944) (0.5199) Thursday −0.0107 0.0223 0.0671** 0.0774 (0.6087) (0.4859) (0.0155) (0.4917) Friday 0.0224 0.0732* 0.0782*** 0.2383** (0.5070) (0.05384) (0.0051) (0.0370) Outcomet −0.1298** −0.5265*** 0.3110** 1.2969** (0.0306) (0.0007) (0.0421) (0.0423) ht 0.0438*** 0.0139* 0.0246 0.0457 (0.0000) (0.0815) (0.5294) (0.2550) Outcomet·ht 0.0969* 0.6350*** −0.6318** −0.6102** (0.0571) (0.0002) (0.0233) (0.0330) PanelB:estimatesfortheEGARCHspecifications

Parameters K 0.0102*** −0.0048 −0.0212** 0.1027*** (0.0051) (0.2605) (0.0120) (0.0000) logh2 t−1 0.9893*** 0.1230*** 0.5426*** 1.0198*** (0.0000) (0.0000) (0.0000) (0.0000) logh2 t−2 0.8470*** 0.4028*** −0.3890** (0.0000) (0.0000) (0.0148) logh2 t−3 0.3224*** (0.0002)



εt−1 ht−1



−E



εt−1 ht−1



+ εt−1 ht−1



0.3015*** 0.1477*** 0.4011*** 0.3638*** (0.0000) (0.0000) (0.0000) (0.0000)



εt−2 ht−2



−E



εt−2 ht−2



+ εt−2 ht−2



−0.0746** 0.1237*** (0.0204) (0.0000)



εt−3 ht−3



−E



εt−3 ht−3



+ εt−3 ht−3



−0.0510* −0.0200 (0.0752) (0.4829)  −0.0939** −0.3877*** 0.0211 −0.0764** (0.0152) (0.0000) (0.5730) (0.0387) TT 4.8537*** 11.1588*** 5.6742*** 7.4441*** (0.0000) (0.0000) (0.0000) (0.0000) Functionvalue −5155.5998 −3952.5773 −2176.8415 −8510.7244 PanelC:Ljung-BoxQ-statistics

Lags

5 0.1118 0.5300 0.0297** 0.0195**

10 0.3229 0.9171 0.1784 0.1531

20 0.0859* 0.8559 0.1642 0.5242

60 0.4049 0.8722 0.2672 0.8138

PanelD:ARCH-LMtests Lags

5 0.9974 0.0016*** 0.0383** 0.4072

10 1.0000 0.0331** 0.1909 0.0152**

20 1.0000 0.1747 0.5037 0.0584*

60 1.0000 0.8863 0.4444 0.2204

PanelE:non-parametric(sign)tests

Signbiastest 0.3570 0.7859 0.0298** 0.3159

Negativesizebiastest 0.4256 0.2170 0.4949 0.4659 Positivesizebiastest 0.4521 0.2515 0.0812* 0.3155

Jointtest 0.3625 0.3856 0.1546 0.3595

Source:Thedataonsoccerscoresarefromhttp://www.rsssf.comandthedataonreturnsfromDatastream.

Wereportp-valuesinparentheses.Theestimatedcoefficientsforthelaggedvaluesofthedependentvariableshavenotbeenreportedtosavespace.

***Indicatesthelevelofsignificanceatthe1%level. ** Indicatesthelevelofsignificanceatthe5%level. * Indicatesthelevelofsignificanceatthe10%level.

that losses have more of an effect on the countries

withmoresuccessfulsoccerteams;however,winshave

more of an effect on countries with less successful

teams.

The estimates for the EGARCH model and a

bat-tery of specification tests for the model are reported

in Table 3’s Panels B–E. We show the estimated

(6)

the countries under consideration, the coefficients of

log(ht−i)2 and



εt−j/ht−j



E



εt−j/ht−j



+t−j/ht−j



are

significantlydifferentfromzero.3 Thelagged-value

esti-matedcoefficientsoflog(h2

t)arestatisticallysignificantand

thecharacteristicrootsofthepolynomialareallinsidethe

unitcircle.Thus,noneoftheconditionalvariance

specifi-cationsisexplosive.Evidenceontheleverageeffect,the

estimated coefficientfor theleverage effect,, is

nega-tiveforSpain,TurkeyandtheUK,indicatingthatnegative

surprisesincreasevolatilitymorethanpositivesurprises.

However,wedonotobserveastatisticallysignificant

lever-ageeffectforChile.

TosetupthemaximumlikelihoodestimatorforEqs.

(1)and (2),weassumethaterrors haveaGeneralError

Distribution.TTistheparameterforreturntailthickness.

ATT>2impliesthaterrorshaveathickerdistributionthan

normal.

Panel C reports the p-values of the Ljung-Box

Q-statistics forthestandardized squaredresiduals, (ε2

t/h2t),

calculatedtotestthenullhypothesisofzero

autocorrela-tionupto60lags.Overall,wecannotrejectthenullofno

autocorrelationforanycountry.Theonlyexceptionsare

Spainat20lagswithasignificancelevelat10%,andChile

andTurkeyatfivelagswithasignificancelevelat5%.

Next, totestfor thenull hypothesisthat there isno

ARCHeffectforthestandardizedresiduals(εt/ht),weapply

Engle’s(1982)ARCH-LMtest.Teststatisticsforthe

stan-dardizedresidualsarereportedinPanelC.Totestthenull

hypothesisthatthereisnoARCHeffect,weregressedthe

squaredstandardizedresidualsonaconstanttermandon

itsfifth,tenth,twentiethandsixtiethlags.Here,theaimis

totestwhetherlagtermsarejointlystatisticallysignificant;

theresultsshowthatthenullofnoARCHeffectisrejected

onlyforChileatfivelagsatthe5%significancelevel,for

Turkeyat10lagsatthe5%significanceleveland20lagsat

the10%significancelevelandfortheUKatfiveand10lags

atthe1%and5%significancelevels,respectively.

PanelEreportsthenon-parametricsignandsizebias

testsperformedforthestandardizedresiduals(εt/ht).

Cal-culatingtheteststatistics,weusestandardizedresiduals

(εt/ht).We addtwo dummyvariables,mt andpt,tothe

equationsuchthatmt=1ifthenormalizedresidualis

neg-ative,0otherwise,andpt=1ifitispositive,0otherwise.

Wealsodefinetwointeractivedummyvariables:smtand

spt.

smt=pt∗(εt/ht) and spt=pt∗(εt/ht)

smt=pt·(εt/ht) and spt=pt·(εt/ht)

Later,weregress(εt/ht)ontheconstantterm,mt,smtand

spt.Inthesigntest,wedeterminewhetherthecoefficient

ofmiszeroornot.Withthenegativesigntest,we test

whetherthecoefficientofsmt iszero;withthepositive

signtestwetestwhetherthecoefficientofspiszeroand

withthejointtestwetestthesenullhypothesesjointly.

3Wedeterminetheselectionofthelagorderforbothtermssuchthat

thespecificationtestsreportedinPanelsB,C,DandEdonotrevealthat standardizedresidualsarenotiid.

Theresultsshowthatnocountry’steststatisticsare

statisti-callysignificant,exceptforthesignbiasedtestandpositive

sizebiastestforChile,whicharestatisticallysignificantat

the5%and10%levels,respectively.Hence,sincemosttest

statisticsreportedinPanelsB–Earenotstatistically

signif-icant,weconcludethattheysupportourspecifications.

4. Conclusions

This paper presents empirical evidence that soccer

matchscoresaffectstockmarketreturnsandstockmarket

return–volatility relationships. Evidence from countries

withrelativelymoresuccessfulsoccer teams,Spainand

theUK,indicate stockmarket returnsdecrease and the

risk–returnrelationshipchangeswiththematchscoreso

thatagentsbecomemoreriskaverseafteraloss.However,

we cannotfind statisticallysignificantevidencefor this

changeafterawin.Thedatafromcountrieswithrelatively

lesssuccessfulsoccerteams,ChileandTurkey,revealthat

stockmarketreturnsincreaseandagentsbecomemorerisk

lovingafterawin.Similarly,wecannotfindstatistical

sig-nificanceafterlosses.Themore-pronouncedlosseffectfor

SpainandtheUKandwineffectforChileandTurkeymaybe

duetosuccessfulhistoriesorfanexpectationsfromthese

countries’soccerteams.

References

Arkes,H.,Herren,L.,&Isen,A.(1988).Theroleofpotentiallossinthe influenceofaffectonrisk-takingbehavior.OrganizationalBehaviorand HumanDecisionProcesses,66,228–236.

Berument,H.,Ceylan,N.B.,&Gozpinar,E.(2006).Performanceofsoccer onthestockmarket:EvidencefromTurkey.SocialScienceJournal,43, 695–699.

Berument,H.,Inamlik,A.,&Yucel,E.(2003).Theeffectoffootballon productivity.IktisatIsletmeveFinans,212,51–62.

Berument,H.,&Yucel,E.(2005).LongliveFenerbahce:Production boost-ingeffectsofsoccerinTurkey.JournalofEconomicPsychology,26, 842–861.

Bollerslev, T. (1986). Generalized autoregressive conditional het-eroskedasticity.JournalofEconometrics,31,307–327.

Cosimano,T.F.,&Jansen,D.W.(1988).EstimatesofthevarianceofU.S. inflationbasedupontheARCHModel.JournalofMoney,Credit& Bank-ing,20(3),409–421.

Dohmen,T.,Falk,A.,Huffman,D.,&Sunde,U.(2006).Seeminglyirrelevant eventsaffecteconomicperceptionsandexpectations:TheFIFAWorldCup 2006asanaturalexperiment.IZADiscussionPapersNo:2275. Edmans,A.,Garcia,D.,&Norli,O.(2007).Sportssentimentandstock

returns.JournalofFinance,62,1967–1998.

Engle,R.F.(1982).Autoregressiveconditionalheteroskedasticitywith estimatesofthevarianceofU.K.inflation.Econometrica,50,987–1008. Hanke,M., & Kirchler, M.(2010). Football championships and Jersey sponsors’stockprices:Anempiricalinvestigation,WorkingPapersin EconomicsandStatistics,UniversityofInnsbruck.

Hirt,E.R.,Erickson,G.A.,Kennedy,C.,&Zillmann,D.(1992).Costsand ben-efitsofallegiance:Changesinfans’self-ascribedcompetenciesafter teamvictoryversusdefeat.JournalofPersonalityandSocialPsychology, 63,724–738.

Johnson,E.J.,&Tversky,A.(1983).Affect,generalization,andthe percep-tionofrisk.JournalofPersonalityandSocialPsychology,45,20–31. Leahy,R.L.(2002).Psychologyandtheeconomicmind:Cognitiveprocesses

&conceptualization.NewYork:SpringerPublishingCompany. Loewenstein,G. (2000).Emotionsineconomic theoryandeconomic

behavior.AmericanEconomicReview,90(2),426–432.

Loewenstein,G.F.,Weber,E.U.,Hsee,C.K.,&Welch,N.(2001).Riskas feelings.PsychologicalBulletin,127(2),267–286.

Nelson,D.(1991).Conditionalheteroskedasticityinassetreturns:Anew approach.Econometrica,59,347–370.

Pollard,P.S.(2002).Grooowwwth!.InternationalEconomicTrends,Annual Edition.July1-1.

(7)

Romer,P.M.(2000).Thinkingandfeeling.AmericanEconomicReview, 90(2),439–443.

Stracca,L.(2004).Behavioralfinanceandassetprices:Wheredowestand? JournalofEconomicPsychology,25(3),373–405.

Wann,D.L.,Dolan,T.J.,Mcgeorge,K.K.,&Allison,J.A.(1994). Relation-shipsbetweenspectatoridentificationandspectators’perceptionsof influence,spectators’emotions,andcompetitionoutcome.Journalof Sport&ExercisePsychology,16,347–364.

Referanslar

Benzer Belgeler

Therefore, this study aimed to determine whether alexithymia related to musculoskeletal pain, job stress, and job and life satisfaction in young academicians working

Karadenizli “Malcı Davut” tüm “sivri ze- kâ”sı ve “açık gözlülüğüyle” bugün Haliç’te keçileriyle başbaşa... Göçüp geldiği, özlemi­ ni

Aşağıdaki noktalı kağıda kenar uzunlukları birer doğal sayı olan ve alanı 30 br 2 olan farklı dikdört-.

Batı Avrupa kahveyi içkiden daha az sevmiyor/Avrupamn büyük otelleri hakkında ge­ çen yüz yılın sonunda çıkan bir'yazıda görmüştüm: Şer­ lindeki Kayzer

Uniform alternate layer calix[8]acid/calix[4]amine LB thin films were prepared using the LB thin film deposition procedure, and the electrical properties for these LB thin films

based knowledge representation.Empirical results show that BMFI has promising performance compared to recent cost-sensitive algorithms in terms of the benefit gained..

First, we report a self-assembly method to prepare PEGylated or peptide functionalized mesoporous silica nanoparticles (MSNs) for cell labeling and drug

Seen from his 17 perspective, the half of the vase that appears includes both the libation scene and the bull sacrifice scene (Figure 29)..