• Sonuç bulunamadı

Parents who want their PreK children to have science learning experiences are outliers

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Parents who want their PreK children to have science learning experiences are outliers"

Copied!
12
0
0

Yükleniyor.... (view fulltext now)

Tam metin

(1)

ContentslistsavailableatScienceDirect

Early

Childhood

Research

Quarterly

Parents

who

want

their

PreK

children

to

have

science

learning

experiences

are

outliers

Mesut

Sac¸

kes

BalıkesirUniversity,NecatibeySchoolofEducation,10100,Balıkesir,Turkey

a

r

t

i

c

l

e

i

n

f

o

Articlehistory:

Received23February2013 Receivedinrevisedform 17November2013 Accepted22November2013 Keywords: Preschool Kindergarten Science Curriculum Parents

a

b

s

t

r

a

c

t

Thepurposeofthisstudywastoexamineparentalprioritiesregardingscienceeducationintheearly

years.Thestudysampleincluded1456Turkishparentsofpre-Kchildren(36–72months).Parentswere

askedtoprioritizeeightacademiccontentareas,includingscience,indescendingorder.Theresults

demonstratedthatthenumberofparentswhoprioritizedscienceoverotheracademiccontentareasin

pre-Kclassroomswasquitelow.Parentswhorankedsciencehighlydeviatedfromthewholesample

andalmostalloftheseparentswereidentifiedasoutliersbythetwo-stepclusteranalysis.Parentsof

boysandyoungerchildrenandparentswithhigh-SESlevelweremorelikelytoprioritizescienceover

otheracademiccontentareas.Thefindingssuggestthatparentalpreferencesoverallalignwellwithearly

childhoodteachers’tendencytoteachlesssciencethanothercontentareas,andparentalprioritiesmight

beanothermajorfactorthatcontributestolimitedsciencelearningexperiencesintheearlyyears.

©2013ElsevierInc.Allrightsreserved.

1. Introduction

Science education in early childhood classrooms has been largelyunderrepresentedinpreschoolcurriculaandearly learn-ingexperiences.Childrenhavefeweropportunitiestolearnscience thanliteracy,mathematics,socialstudies,andartsintypicalearly childhoodclassrooms(Bracken&Fischel,2006;Earlyetal.,2010; Greenfield et al., 2009; Nayfeld, Brenneman, & Gelman, 2011; Sac¸kes,Trundle,Bell,&O’Connell,2011;Varol,2013).Mostearly childhood teachersteach science onlyonce or twiceperweek, resultinginverylimitedtimeforsciencelearning(Sac¸kesetal., 2011)andteacherstendtofocusonteachinglifescienceconcepts morethanphysicalandearthandspacescienceconcepts(Sac¸kes, inpress).

Thereasonsforthelimitedvisibilityofsciencelearning experi-encesinearlychildhoodclassroomstodatehavebeenexamined solelyfromtheteacher’sperspectives.These studieshave iden-tifiedseveralfactorsthatpreventearlychildhoodteachersfrom providingeffective sciencelearning experiencesfor young chil-dren. Studies have demonstrated that several factors influence earlychildhoodteachers’decisionstodevotelesstimeinteaching scienceintheirclassrooms.Thesefactorsinclude:(1)limited sci-enceandpedagogicalcontentknowledge(Appleton,1992;Kallery &Psillos,2001;Sac¸kes,inpress),(2)pressuretoteachcontentareas otherthanscience(Greenfieldetal.,2009;Nayfeldetal.,2011),

∗ Correspondingauthor.Tel.:+902662412762. E-mailaddress:msackes@gmail.com

(3) limitedavailability of science-related materials(Appleton& Kindt,1999;Earlyetal.,2010;Greenfieldetal.,2009),(4) percep-tionsthatyoungchildrenareincapableoflearningscienceconcepts (Fleer, 2009;Sac¸kes, inpress), (5) and lowteacher self-efficacy for teachingscience(Appleton, 1995;Garbett, 2003;Greenfield etal.,2009;Kallery&Psillos,2001;Maier,Greenfield,& Bulotsky-Shearer,2013;Nayfeldetal.,2011;Pell&Jarvis,2003).

Anincreasingnumberofstudieshaveexaminedhowoftenearly childhood teachers teach scienceconcepts and thefactors that influenceteachers’decisions toteach scienceintheearlyyears (Sac¸kes,in press).Studiessuggest thatparentalsupportfor sci-encelearninginschoolandathomepromoteschildren’sinterest inscienceandconceptualunderstandingofscientificphenomena (Alexander, Johnson, & Kelly, 2012; Mantzicopoulos, Patrick, & Samarapungavan, 2013; Tenenbaum, Snow, Roach, & Kurland, 2005).Positiveexperienceswithlearningscienceinschoolmight influenceparents’beliefsaboutteachingandlearningsciencein theearlyyears.Parentswithaffirminglearningexperiencesmight have a positive attitude toward learning science and have the resourcestosupporttheirownchildren’slearningofscience(Chen, 2001;Dierking&Falk,1994;Ferry,Fouad,&Smith,2000;George &Kaplan,1998;Kaya&Lundeen,2010;Zady&Portes,2001). Par-ents’beliefsabouttheimportanceandappropriatenessoflearning sciencemayinfluencethequalityandthequantityofscience learn-ingopportunitieschildrenreceiveathomeandinschool.However, parents’perceptionsoftheimportanceofsciencelearningin com-parisontootheracademiccontentareashavenotbeenexaminedin theliterature.Therefore,thecurrentstudyaimstofillthegapinthe literatureviaexaminingparents’preferencesforlearningscience

0885-2006/$–seefrontmatter©2013ElsevierInc.Allrightsreserved.

(2)

inearlychildhoodclassroomscomparedtootheracademiccontent areas.

1.1. Parents’prioritiesforpreschoolandkindergartencurriculum Parental prioritiesfor earlychildhood curriculum havebeen extensively studied in theliterature. For example, the findings ofthestudiesconductedinHongKong, Australia,andU.S.have demonstratedthatparentsaremorelikelythanearlychildhood teacherstofocus onacademic learning(Dockett&Perry, 2004; Ebbeck,1995;Knudsen-Lindauer&Harris,1989).Morespecifically, parentsareinclinedtorankreading,writing,andmathematicsas beingmoreimportantthandoteachersofyoungchildren(Hewitt &Maloney,2000; Knudsen-Lindauer& Harris,1989).American fatherstend toemphasize academic learning more than moth-ers and parents’academic expectationsfor girls werefoundto behigherthanboys(McBride&Ferguson,1992).Parentsofolder preschoolerstendtoperceiveacademicskillsasmoreimportant thantheparentsofyoungerpreschoolersinHongKong(Fung& Cheng,2012).Likewise,parents with low socioeconomicstatus and minorityparents tendtofocus moreonacademic learning thannon-minorityparentsandthoseofhighersocioeconomic sta-tuses(Evans&Fuller,1998;Harding,2006;Kernan&Hayes,1999; Ojala,2000;Piotrkowski,Botsko,&Matthews,2000).Researchers alsohave identifiedcross-culturaldifferences inparents’ expec-tationsandpriorities.ForexampleAmerican,Chinese,Malaysian, andIrishparentsofpre-Kchildrentendtofocusmoreonacademic learning than Swedish,Finnish,Icelandic, and Japanese parents (Einarsdóttir,2010;Hewitt&Maloney,2000;Ojala,2000;Tobin, Wu,&Davidson,1991).Children’sacquisitionofmoralandethical valuesand behaviorsalsoisconsideredasanimportant educa-tionalgoalintheearlyyears(Brownleeetal.,2012;LePageetal., 2011;Luo,Tamis-LeMonda,&Song,2013).Turkishparentsexpect theirchildrentoacquiremoralvaluesearlyintheirlives(Nacak, Ya˘gmurlu,Durgel,&vandeVijver,2011).Thefindingsofarecent studywithTurkishparentsofpreschoolagechildrendemonstrated thatamajorityoftheparentsexpectsschoolstoprovide system-aticmoraleducationinpreschool classrooms(Balat,Beceren,& Özdemir,2011).

Althoughparentsofpre-Kchildren perceivesocial and emo-tional development as important areas to be targeted in early childhoodclassrooms,theytendtofavorcognitivedevelopment andrelatedacademicskillsmorethanotherareas.Evenparents whoreportthattheirchildren’ssocialemotionaldevelopmentis theprimaryreasonthattheysendthemtopreschooltendtoprefer activitiesthatsupportacademiclearninginpre-Kclassrooms,and theyperceiveacademicandcognitivedevelopmentastheprimary outcomesofpreschooleducation(Hewitt&Maloney,2000).

Instudiesthatexamineparents’expectationsandprioritiesfor earlychildhoodprograms,reading,writing,andmathematicsare theonly academic skills parents aretypically asked torank or rateamongalistofotherskills(Kernan&Hayes,1999; Knudsen-Lindauer&Harris,1989).Nopreviousstudiesincludedscienceas anoptionforparentstoconsiderorexaminedhowparents pri-oritizescienceamongotheracademiccontentareas.Thefindings ofonlyonerecentstudyprovideaglimpseofwhatparentsmight thinkabouttheplaceofsciencelearninginearlychildhood curricu-lum.Whiletheliteracyandmathematicsactivitieswerethemost preferredlearningexperiencesparentswantedfortheirchildren, activitiesthathaveapotentialtopromotescientificthinkingsuch assandplayandwaterplayweretheleastpreferredactivitiesby theMalaysianparentssurveyedinthatstudy(Hewitt&Maloney, 2000).

Greenfieldetal.(2009)suggestedthatpreschoolscience edu-cationisanemergingresearchareaandbasicstudiesthatprovide preliminaryempiricaldatashouldbeconducted.Theseresearchers

suggestthatfutureresearchinthis areashouldfocuson under-standingthereasons scienceis not targetedin earlychildhood classroomsandproposethatteacher-relatedvariables,suchas self-efficacyandtime-constraints,anddevelopinginstrumentstoassess preschoolsciencereadinessandqualityshouldbemajorresearch topics (Greenfield et al., 2009). A neglected factor in this pro-posedresearchagendaisanexaminationofparents’expectations andprioritiesforscienceinearlychildhoodprograms.Although earlychildhoodteachers’beliefs,theirclassroompractices,andthe child-relatedfactorsareimportantcomponents,parental involve-ment,expectations,andprioritiesregardingscienceeducationin theearlyyearsshouldalsobeincludedinsuchanagenda(Ginsburg &Golbeck,2004).

1.2. Earlychildhoodscienceeducation

A large body of literature provides evidence that children developideas,althoughmostlydivergentfromscientificaccounts, abouthowthenaturalworldworksveryearlyintheirlives.For example, studiesrevealedthat children have naïveideas about night and day cycle,the shape of the earth, themoon phases, distinctionbetweenlivingandnonlivingthings,propertiesof phys-ical objects, light and shadows, and the mechanism of rainfall (Carey,1985;Hannust&Kikas,2007;Hobson,Trundle,&Sackes, 2010;Krnel,Watson,&Glazar,2005;Sac¸kes,Flevares,&Trundle, 2010;Segal&Cosgrove,1993;Venville,2004;Vosniadou&Brewer, 1992;Vosniadou&Brewer,1994).Collectively,findingsfromthese studiesdemonstratedthatevenpreschoolerscometoschoolwith intuitive understandings of various natural phenomena. There-fore,researcherssuggestthateducationalexperiencesforyoung children should take their prior knowledge into account and provideopportunitiestohelpchildren inrestructuringand con-structingtheirconceptualunderstandingsofsciencephenomena (Harlen,2001;Osborne&Freyberg,1985;Trundle&Sac¸kes,2012; Vosniadou,Ioannides,Dimitrakopoulou,&Papademetriou,2001).

Scienceeducationinearlychildhoodclassroomsshouldprovide opportunitiesforchildrentodeveloptheirscientificthinkingskills, conceptual understanding of natural phenomena, and positive attitudestoward science(Sac¸keset al.,2011;Trundle&Sac¸kes, 2012).Studies suggest that children’sunderstanding of science conceptsand theirscientificthinkingskillsdevelopsuccessively (Duschl,Schweingruber,&Shouse,2007;Hmelo-Silver&Duncan, 2009). Early exposure to developmentally appropriate science learningopportunitiesmaypromotechildren’slearningofmore advancedscienceconceptsandinquiryskillsinsubsequentyears (Sac¸kesetal.,2011;Tao,Oliver,&Venville,2012)andhelp chil-drendeveloppositiveattitudestowardscience(Mantzicopoulos, Patrick, & Samarapungavan, 2008; Patrick, Mantzicopoulos, & Samarapungavan,2009),whichislinkedtolaterscience achieve-ment and STEM-related career choices (Maltese & Tai, 2010; Osborne,Simon,&Collins,2003).

Contemporaryscienceeducationliteratureemphasizes inquiry-basedinstructionasoneofthemosteffectivewaystopromote conceptual understandings of scientific phenomena (Anderson, 2002,2007; Trundle& Sac¸kes,2012).Inquiry-basedinstruction withPre-Kchildren takes children’sexisting conceptual under-standingintoconsiderationandincludeshands-onactivitieswith sense-makingdiscussions where children areencouragedto be physically,mentally,andemotionallyactive(Sac¸kesetal.,2011). Inquiry-based instruction with Pre-K children should involve developmentallyappropriatestructuringandscaffoldingsuchas informulatingresearchquestionsandprocedurestoreducethe amountofcognitiveloadthatmayrestrainchildrenfrom construc-tingconceptualunderstandings and developing scienceprocess skills(Mayer,2004;Nayfeldetal.,2011;Peterson&French,2008; Trundle&Sac¸kes,2012).Insuchalearningenvironment,children

(3)

are more likely to develop scientific inquiry skills, construct scientificunderstandingsaboutnaturalphenomena,andbuildan interestinandapositiveattitudetowardscience(Mantzicopoulos et al., 2013; Miller, Smith, Trundle,Sac¸kes, & Mollohan, 2013; Peterson&French,2008;).Inquiry-basedscienceinstructioncan beeasilyintegratedwithplay,whichisconsideredasaneffective and developmentally appropriate pedagogical tool for young learners(Baldwin,Adams,&Kelly,2009;Bodrova&Leong,2003; Fleer,2011;Trawick-Smith,2012).Althoughempiricalevidence regardingtheeffectivenessofplayinsupportingyoungchildren’s learningofscienceis scarce,preliminaryevidencesuggeststhat inquiry-based play activities may have a potential to promote youngchildren’sconceptualunderstandingsofsciencephenomena (Blake&Howitt,2012;Fleer,2009;Milleretal.,2013).

Inquiry-basedinstructiontoteachingscienceispossiblyquite differentthanwhatmostparentshaveexperiencedintheirformal schooling,whichtypicallyincludestraditionalordidactic instruc-tion(Kaya&Lundeen,2010).Intraditionalformsofinstruction, whereconceptsarepresentedinfinalformviaverbalexplanations andlectures,childrenremainpassiverecipientoftheinformation (Trundle,Atwood,Christopher,&Sac¸kes,2010).Thistypeof learn-ingenvironment,unlikeinquiry-basedinstruction,doesnotoffer opportunitiesforchildrentotest,reflect,andcomparetheir exist-ingconceptualunderstandingwithwhatemergesintheclassroom (Mayer,2004;Trundleetal.,2010;Vosniadouetal.,2001).Parents wholearnedsciencethroughtraditionalformsofinstructionmay perceivethelearningofscienceasthememorizationofscientific factsandformulas.Consequently,mostparentsofyoungchildren mightbelievethatteachingscienceinpre-Kclassroomsis develop-mentallyinappropriate.Indeed,studiessuggestthatparentstend toprioritizeacademiccontentareaslikereadingandwritingand donot preferactivitiesthat havea potentialtopromote scien-tificthinking(Hewitt&Maloney,2000;Knudsen-Lindauer&Harris, 1989).Therefore,inthisstudysciencewaspredictedtobetheleast preferredacademiccontentareabyparents,andparentswho pri-oritizesciencewereexpectedtodeviatefromthewholesample andbeidentifiedasoutliersintheanalysis.

1.3. Parentsandearlyscienceexperiences

Parentshelppreschoolchildrendevelopanearly understand-ingofnaturalphenomenaandbasicscienceprocessskills(Harris &Koenig,2006;Siegel,Esterly,Callanan,Wright,&Navarro,2007). Parentspromoteyoungchildren’sunderstandingofbiology con-cepts(Cumming,2003;Jipson&Callanan,2003;Rigney&Callanan, 2011), physicalscienceconcepts(Tenenbaum&Callanan,2008; Tenenbaum,Snow,Roach,&Kurland,2005)andastronomy con-cepts(Callanan,Jipson,&Soennichsen,2002)aswellascontribute tochildren’sunderstandingofthedomaindifferences,thatis, bio-logicaland astronomicalphenomena fallintodifferentdomains ofscience(Jipson&Callanan,2003).Duringparent-child interac-tionsathomeandininformalsciencelearningsettings,parents provideguidance andcausal explanationstosupportchildren’s understanding of natural phenomena and the development of theirscientificreasoning.Duringinformalscienceactivities, par-entspurposefullychangethetypeofexplanationstheyprovideto accommodatechild’sageandactivitytype.Forexample,parents becomemoredirective withyoungerchildren (threeyears old) duringstructuredactivitiesandwhenmakingpredictions. How-ever,theybecome more collaborative witholderchildren (five yearsold)duringopen-endedactivitiesandwhentestingtheir pre-dictions(Siegeletal., 2007).Parentsof boystend toencourage moredetailedscientificreasoningintheirsons,andtheyprovide boysmoreopportunitiestorespondindiscussionsofscientific con-ceptsthanparentsofgirls(Crowley,Callanan,Tenenbaum,&Allen, 2001;Tenenbaum&Leaper,2003;Tenenbaumetal.,2005).Early

sciencelearningopportunitiesprovidedathomeandparental sup-portforsciencelearningarelikelytofosterchildren’sinterestin science,contributechildren’slearningofscience,andpromotetheir comprehension of scientific discourse in early childhood class-rooms and beyond (Alexander et al., 2012; Tenenbaum et al., 2005).

Byhelpingchildren makesenseofthenaturalworldaround themandbyprovidingrelevanteducationalexperiences,parents functionasaprimaryfacilitatorofyoungchildren’searlyscience learningexperiences. Althoughabody ofliteraturehas demon-stratedtherole parentsplay in offeringandenriching informal sciencelearningexperiencesfor youngchildren,ourknowledge ofwhatparentsthinkaboutformalsciencelearningexperiencesin earlychildhoodclassroomisverylimited.

1.4. Turkishcontext

InTurkey,earlychildhoodeducationservicesaremainly over-seenbytheTurkishMinistryofNationalEducation,andwithfew exceptions,almostalloftheseservicesarecenter-based(Bekman, 2005; Bekman & Göc¸er, 2005; Kagitcibasi, Sunar, & Bekman, 2001).Startingfromthe1990s,earlychildhoodeducationgained considerable attentionas indicated by ongoing efforts for pro-gramdevelopmentanddisseminationofearlyeducationservices (Bekman,2005).Theteacherandtopic-centeredearlychildhood programof1989haveturnedintochild-centereddevelopmental programsduringtheyearsof1994,2002and2006(Can-Yasar,Inal, Uyanik,&Kandir,2011;Güler-Öztürk,2010).Anewprogramwas developedandpilotedduringthe2012–2013academicyear,and theprogramwillbeimplementedinsubsequentyears(Ministryof NationalEducation[MEB],2012).Adecadeago,theschoolingrate forearlychildhoodeducationwasabout11%(Bekman,2005).Since then,theschoolingratehasrisento26.63%andcurrentlyabout 1,077,933children(3–5yearsold)benefitfrompublicorprivate earlychildhoodeducationservices(MEB,2013;TurkishStatistical Institute[TÜ˙IK],2013).

Althoughanumberofstateandnon-governmental organiza-tionssponsored campaignefforts hastaken placeover thelast decadetoraisepublicawarenessofearlychildhoodeducationand toincreaseschoolingrates,preschooleducationstillisnotapartof compulsoryeducationinTurkey(Bekman&Gürlesel,2005;MEB, 2011).Eventhoughpubliclyfundedpreschoolsarefreeoftuition charges,parentsneedtopayfortransportationandnutritionfees, whichvaryfromschooltoschoolandmaycostasmuchas$150 permonth(MEB,2004;WorldBank,2013).Consideringthecurrent legalminimumwagesofapproximately$400permonth,mostlow incomefamiliescannotaffordtoenrolltheirchildreninpreschool. Therefore,childrenoflowincomefamilies,thosewhoneedearly educationservicesmost,arelesslikelytoreceivepreschool educa-tionthantheirpeersfromhigherincomefamilies(Bekman,2005; WorldBank,2013).

TheTurkishearlychildhood educationsystemsuffersfroma highchild-to-teacherratio,unbalanceddistributionofresources, inadequate funding, a lack of diversity inthe types ofservices offered,andlowprocessquality(Bas¸türk&Is¸ıko˘glu,2008;Bekman, 2002,2005;Derman&Bas¸al,2010;Kalkan&Akman,2009;World Bank,2013).Thechild-to-teacherratioismuchhigherinTurkey thaninothercountriesliketheU.S.andSweden,anditissimilar tocountrieslikeKoreaandChina(McMullenetal.,2005;Sheridan, Giota,Han,&Kwon,2009).AsinPortugal,SwedenandGermany, self-careskillsandmotorskillsarehighlyemphasizedinTurkish earlychildhood classrooms,whereasdiversityand multicultural values are not commonly addressed (Bas¸türk &Is¸ıko˘glu,2008; Sac¸kes,2013;Sheridanetal.,2009;Tietze,Cryer,Bairrão,Palacios, &Wetzel,1996).SimilartotheU.S.,Finland,andKorea,variations inthequalityoftheservicesofferedinTurkishearlychildhood

(4)

educationinstitutionsappeartobehigh(Bekman,2002;Hujala, Fonsén,&Elo,2012;Sheridanetal.,2009;Tietzeetal.,1996).Like mostEuropeancountries,abachelor’sdegreeisrequiredtoserve asanearlychildhoodeducationteacherinpubliclyfundedTurkish preschools(Kayhan&Kılıc¸,2011).Yet,highschoolgraduatesand associatedegreeholdersinchilddevelopmentcommonlyservein institutionsnotoverseenbytheMinistryofNationalEducation, andthisdifferenceinteachertrainingcontributestotheobserved variationsinthequalityoftheearlyeducationservices(Bekman, 2005).

Turkishearlychildhoodteachers’beliefsaboutteachingyoung children appear to be aligned well with the Developmentally AppropriatePracticesoftheNationalAssociationfortheEducation ofYoungChildren(NAEYC)(Erdiller,2013;Erdiller&McMullen, 2004).However,thecongruencebetweenTurkishearlychildhood teachers’beliefsandtheirclassroompracticesisweakerthantheir colleaguesintheU.S.,Taiwan,andKorea,buthigherthanChina (McMullenetal.,2005).Turkishearlychildhoodteachers’actual classroompracticesdeviatefromtheDevelopmentallyAppropriate PracticesofNAEYCforseveralreasons:limitedphysicalresources, highchild-to-teacherratio,limitedperceivedparentalandcollegial support,andthecollectivisttoneofTurkishculture,ormoreaptly cultureofrelatedness(Erdiller,2013;Erdiller&McMullen,2004; Kagitcibasi,2005;McMullenetal.,2005).

LikeSwedenandSouthKorea,Turkeyhasanationalearly child-hoodeducationprogramandearlyeducationservicesareregulated bythecentralizedgovernment(Sheridanetal.,2009).TheTurkish earlychildhood educationprogram isadevelopmentalprogram and theprogram doesnot include earlylearning content stan-dardsforthepreschoolandkindergartenlevel(Sac¸kesetal.,2010). Theprogramtargetsthedevelopmentofself-careskillsand socio-emotional,cognitive,motor,andlanguagedevelopmentofchildren (MEB,2012).Eventhoughthedevelopmentofsomescience pro-cessskills,suchasmakingobservationsandestablishingacause andeffectrelationship,aretargetedintheprogram,theprogram doesnotexplicitlyfocusondevelopingconceptualunderstandings ofbasicscienceconceptsinchildren.

IntypicalTurkishpreschoolandkindergartenclassrooms,very littletimeisallocatedforscienceactivities(Ayvacı,Devecio˘glu,& Yi˘git,2002;Özbey&Alisinano˘glu,2008;Varol,2013).When Turk-ishearlychildhoodeducatorsdoteachsciencetheymostlyfocus onspecificlife and spacescience conceptsand utilizeteaching strategiesthatarecontrarytothecontemporary science educa-tionliterature(Ayvacıetal.,2002;Güler&Bıkmaz,2002;Özbey &Alisinano˘glu,2008;Sı˘gırtmac¸&Özbek,2011).Teachers’limited scienceand pedagogical content knowledgecoupledwith their lowself-confidenceforteachingyoungchildrenscienceappearto bemajorbarrierstoprovidingdevelopmentally appropriate sci-encelearningopportunitiesinTurkishpre-Kclassrooms(Ayvacı etal.,2002;Özbey&Alisinano˘glu,2008;Sac¸kes,Akman,&Trundle, 2012).

1.5. FactorsInfluencingParents’Priorities

Resultsofpreviousstudiesdemonstratedthatseveralfactors influenceparentalexpectationsandpriorities.Pastexperiencesare likelytoinfluenceparents’expectationsandprioritiesforpre-K curriculum(Evans&Fuller,1998).Parentswitholderchildrenwho hadpreviouspre-Kexperiencesmighthavedifferentprioritiesfor theirchildrenthanparentswithnosuchexperience(Joesch,Maher, &Durfee,2006; Palacios,1990).Parents’levelof educationand incomealsoarelikelytoinfluencetheirexpectationsand priori-tiesforpre-Kcurricula.Parentsmighthavedifferentexpectations forboysandgirls,andparents’expectationsmightdifferbasedon theirchildren’sages(Chang,Sandhofer,&Brown,2011;Crowley etal.,2001;Fung&Cheng,2012;McBride&Ferguson,1992).Also,

parents’residencystatus(i.e.,ruralorurban)islikelytoinfluence theirexpectationsforpre-Kcurricula(Nacaketal.,2011;Palacios, 1990).Parents in rural areas have limited access to resources, suchashighqualityschools,museums,libraries,and observato-riesthatsupportthedevelopmentandeducationoftheirchildren (Aydın,Sarıer, &Uysal, 2012; Shelley & Yildirim,2013; Tansel, 2002). Limited educational and cultural opportunities in rural areasappear tobeassociatedwithchildren’slow performances inlanguageandliteracyskillsandlowereducationalattainment (OrganizationforEconomicCo-operationandDevelopment,2013; Tansel,2002;Ulubas¸o˘glu&Cardak,2007).Anunbalanced distri-butionofeducationalandculturalresourcesbetweenurbanand ruralareasmightleadtovariationsinparentalpreferencesfor aca-demiccontentareas.Parentswhoresideinruralareasmightbe morelikely toprioritizetheacquisitionofknowledgeandskills (i.e.,literacy),whichareperceivedasfundamentalforsuccessin earlygrades,andtheymayplacelessimportanceonacademic con-tentareas(i.e.,science),whichareassociatedwithsuccessinlater grades.Basedontheresultsofthesepreviousstudies,analysisof variancewasusedtoexaminetheextenttowhich parental pri-orities for pre-Kcurricula variedbyhavingan olderchild with previouspre-Kexperiences,thenumberofchildreninthe house-hold,parents’genderandlevelofeducation,thefamily’sresidency status, and monthly household income, children’s gender and age.

1.6. Purposeofthestudy

Thisstudyaimedtoinvestigateparents’perceptionsoftheplace ofscienceeducationinearlychildhoodcurriculumandthe socio-demographicfactorsassociatedwithparentalpriorities.Basedon previousstudiesonparentalprioritiesfortheearlychildhood cur-riculum,thenumberofparentswhoprioritizescienceoverother academiccontentareasfortheirchildrenwaspredictedtobe sub-stantiallylower.Also,parentswhoprioritizesciencearepredicted todeviatefromthewholesampleandbeidentifiedasparentswith anunusualresponsepatternintheanalysis.

2. Method

2.1. Participants

Thesampleofthisstudyincluded1456parentsofpreschool and kindergarten children (36–72 months) located in a mid-size province in the northwestern part of Turkey. Almost 63% oftheparticipantswerefromthedistrictsoftheprovince(and about37% of theparticipantswere fromtheprovincial capital, BalıkesirCity,whichcloselycorrespondstothecensusdata(TÜ˙IK, 2012).

Themajorityoftherespondentsweremothers(73.5%),while 26.5%oftheparticipantswerefathers.Themeanageformothers was33years(Median=32years)andthemeanageforthefathers was37years (Median=36years).Slightlymorethanonefourth (27.2%)oftheparentswereelementaryschoolgraduates,35.6%had ahighschooldegree,9%hadanassociate’sdegree,and28.3%had acollegeorpostgraduatedegree.Theaveragemonthlyhousehold incomereportedbytheparentswas2000TL($1111USD)witha medianof1600TL($888USD).Lessthantwentypercent(17.2%) oftheparentshadchildrenwhowerethreeyearsold,33.8%ofthe parentshadfour-year-oldchildren,and49%oftheparentshad chil-drenwhowerefiveyearsold.Slightlymorethanhalf(51.4%)ofthe childrenwereboyswhile48.6%weregirls.Mostparentshada sin-glechild(50.7%),43.2%hadtwochildren,andafewparents(6.1%) hadthreeormorechildren.

(5)

2.2. Datacollection

Aquestionnairewasdevelopedbasedonpreviousresearchand experts’opinionstoexamineparentalprioritiesforpre-K curricu-lum(Kernan&Hayes,1999;Knudsen-Lindauer&Harris, 1989). A panel of experts with postgraduate degrees in the fields of curriculumandinstruction,earlychildhoodeducation,and psy-chologywereconsultedtodeterminetheacademiccontentareas andestablishthefacevalidityoftheinstrument.Readabilityandthe understandabilitywereexamined,andtheinstrumentwaspiloted with10 parents beforedata collection.Eightacademic content areaswereselectedforinclusionintheinstrument,including math-ematics,arts,socialstudies,science,moralsandethics,pre-reading, pre-writing,andsecondlanguage.Inpreviousstudiesthat exam-inedparentalprioritiesforlearningexperiencesinearlychildhood programs,parentsaretypicallyaskedtorankorrateamongalist oflearningexperiences,activities,contentareas,orskills(Hewitt& Maloney,2000;Kernan&Hayes,1999;Knudsen-Lindauer&Harris, 1989).Asimilarprocedurewasadoptedinthepresentstudy. Par-entswereaskedtoprioritizetheseeightacademiccontentareas indescendingorderbyrankingthemostimportantacademic con-tentareaasthefirstandrankingtheleastimportantastheeighth onthequestionnaireform.Morespecifically,parentswereaskedto respondtothefollowingquestionwhichwasaccompaniedbyalist ofacademiccontentareasinalphabeticalorder:“Whichacademic contentareasshouldhavepriorityinyourchild’spre-Kclassroom? Pleaserankthebelow academicdomainsinthetablebygiving 1tothemostimportantand8 totheleastimportantacademic contentareas.” Thequestionnairealsoincludedquestionsabout parents’socio-demographiccharacteristics.Initially, administra-torsofeducationalinstitutionsservingpre-Kchildrenwithinthe BalıkesirProvincewerecontactedviaaninvitationletterthat pro-videsinformationaboutthestudy.Atthetimeofthestudy,there were32schools(publicandprivate)servingsolelypre-Kchildren inBalıkesirProvinceand23schoolsagreedtoparticipateinthe study.Also,aconveniencesampleof25elementaryschoolswith kindergartenclassroomswasincludedinthestudy.Theschools thatagreedtoparticipateinthestudygrantedpermissiontocollect datafromtheparents.Then,thequestionnaireformsalongwitha letteraboutthestudyweredistributedtoparentswhovolunteered toparticipateinthestudyandthecompletedformswereretrieved withintwotosixweeksofdistribution.

2.3. Dataanalysis

In thepresent study, a cluster analysisprocedure wasused to reveal clusters of parents with similar preferences for aca-demicdomainsinthedataset.Clusteranalysisisamathematical methodofprofilingparticipantswithsimilarpatternsofresponses (Aldenderfer &Blashfield, 1984;Hair,Black, Babin,Anderson,& Tatham,2006).Clusteranalysisisanexploratorymethodthataims tounearththenaturalgroupingsinherentinthedata(Jain,2010). Likefactoranalysis,clusteranalysisaimstodetectpatternsinthe data,butclusteranalysisfocusesongroupingrespondentsrather thanvariables(Hairetal.,2006).Thisstudyseekstoidentify homo-geneous groups of parents with similarpriorities for academic contentareas.Therefore,clusteranalysiswasemployedinthe anal-ysisofthestudysample.Two-stepclusteranalysiswasusedasthe mainanalyticaltoolduetoitsuniqueadvantagesoverother cluster-ingmethods.Two-stepclusteranalysiscanhandlebothcontinuous andcategoricalvariables(includingdichotomous,ordinalandrank ordered),anditperformswellwithlargesamples,hasamechanism tohandleoutliers,andautomaticallyselectsthenumberofclusters (Chiu,Fang,Chen,Wang,&Jeris,2001;Norusis,2012; ˇRezanková, 2009;Yu,2010).

Parentswereclusteredusingthelog-likelihooddistance mea-sure,whichisasuitableclusteringmethodforthedatainthisstudy (Kaufmann&Rousseeuw,2005; ˇRezanková,2009).Thenumberof clusterswasautomaticallydeterminedbythesoftwareusingthe BayesianInformationCriterion(BIC)asaclusteringcriterion.The outlierhandlingoptionwasusedtocreateaseparateclusterfor parentsthatdidnotfitwellintoanyothercluster.The observa-tionswererearrangedinrandomorderbeforetheclusteranalysis wasperformed.Followingtheclusteranalysis,chisquare analy-sisandanalysisof variancewereused toexamine whetherthe parentalprioritiesdiffersignificantlyacrosstheclustersaswellas therelationshipbetweensocio-demographicvariablesandcluster membership.SPSSversion19wasusedinthedataanalysis. 2.4. Missingdata

Initially, thedata setwas examinedfor missing values. The percentageofmissingvalueswaslessthan1%forthedependent variables(parents’ratingsofacademiccontentareas).Missing val-uesinthedependentvariableswereimputedusingtheHotDeck imputation technique (Myers,2011). The Hot Deck imputation techniqueproducesimputationsthatdonotfalloutsidetherange ofpossiblevalues,whichmakesitmoreappropriatethan model-basedstrategiesofdealingwithmissingdata,suchasmaximum likelihood andexpectation-maximization, inestimating missing categoricalandrankorderedobservations(Myers,2011;Siddique &Belin,2008).Thepercentageofmissingvalueswasbetween1.4% to4.1% for theindependent variables (socio-demographic vari-ables),exceptforchildage(7.6%)andtheincomevariable(16.7%). Missingvaluesinthecategoricalindependentvariableswerealso imputedwithHotDeckimputationtechnique.Thechildageandthe incomevariables,however,wereimputedusingthe expectation-maximization(EM)algorithm(Jöreskog&Sörbom,2006).Allcases withmissingvaluesweresuccessfullyimputed.Theanalyseswere performedontheimputeddataset.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptivestatistics

Initially, parents’ ranking preferences were examined using descriptivestatistics.Thepercentagesofparents’rankingsofthe academiccontentareasareillustratedinFig.1.Results demon-stratedthatmoralsandethicswasthemostfrequentlyfirst-ranked area.Over one-thirdof theparents(34.3%)selectedmorals and ethicsastheirfirstcurricularchoice,andthepercentageof par-entswhorankedmoralandethicswithintheirfirstthreechoices was55.2%.

Aboutone-fifth oftheparents(21.1%) rankedpre-readingas themostimportantacademiccontentarea,andalmosttwo-thirds oftheparents’rankedpre-readingwithintheirfirstthreechoices (66.3%).Pre-writingwasanotherofthemosthighlyrankedareas bytheparents.Slightlylowerthanone-fifthoftheparents(18.8%) rankedpre-writingasthemostimportantacademicskill,andas withthepre-reading,almosttwo-thirdsoftheparentsranked pre-writingwithintheirfirstthreechoices(65.9%).

Althoughmathematicswasrankedbyonly10.9%of the par-entsastheirfirstchoice,thepercentageofparentswhoranked mathematicswithintheirfirstthreechoiceswassubstantial(40%). Pre-reading,pre-writing,moralsandethics,andmathematicswere themosthighlyrankedcurricularareasamongtheparentsinthis study.Fig.2illustratesthepercentagesfortopthreerankings.

Sciencewastheleastpreferredacademiccontentareabythe parents.A sizeablenumber ofparents (997participants,68.4%) rankedsciencewithintheirlastthreechoices(6th,7th,and8th).

(6)

34.3 6.3 14.6 11 8.6 7.4 7.2 10.6 0 10 20 30 40 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Percentages Rankings

Ethics and

Mo

ral

21.1 28.5 16.8 10.6 7.3 6.2 4.7 4.9 0 10 20 30 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Percentages Rankings

Pre-

Reading

18.8 30.1 17 11.2 7.6 4.9 6.5 3.9 0 10 20 30 40 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Percentages Rankings

Pre-

Wring

10.9 7.5 21.6 19.5 17 11.6 7.8 4.1 0 5 10 15 20 25 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Percentages Rankings

Mathem

acs

8 7.1 11.6 15.3 19 12 11.2 15.8 0 5 10 15 20 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Percentages Rankings

Second Langua

ge

5.4 12.6 8.9 12.6 13 14.8 14 18.8 0 5 10 15 20 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Percentages Rankings

Art

1.4 5.6 5.9 8.7 13.1 20.3 25.9 19 0 10 20 30 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Percentages Rankings

Social Studies

0.1 2.4 3.6 11.1 14.4 22.8 22.5 23.1 0 5 10 15 20 25 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Percentages Rankings

Science

Fig.1.Percentagesofparents’rankingsofacademiccontentareas.

34.3 21.1 18.8 10.9 8 5.4 1.4 0.1 6.3 28.5 30.1 7.5 7.1 12.6 5.6 2.4 14.6 16.8 17 21.6 11.6 8.9 5.9 3.6 55.2 66.3 65.9 40 26.7 26.9 12.9 6 Morals and Ethics

Pre-Reading Pre-Wring Mathemacs Second

Language

Arts Social

Studies

Science

1st 2nd 3rd Within First Three

(7)

Onlyoneparentrankedscienceasthemostimportantacademic contentareaandthepercentageofparentswhorankedscience withintheirfirstthreechoiceswasmerely6%.Followingscience, socialstudieswasthesecondleastpreferredarea.Asizeable num-berofparents(950participants,65.2%)rankedsocialstudieswithin theirlastthreechoices,andonly12.9%oftheparentsrankedsocial studieswithin theirfirstthree choices. More than a quarterof theparticipantsrankedarts(26.9%)andsecondlanguage(26.7%) withintheirfirstthreechoices.

3.2. ComparingtheTopandtheBottomScienceRankers

Parentswhorankedsciencewithintheirtopthreeandbottom threechoiceswerecomparedbasedontheirchild’sgender,age, andsocioeconomicstatus(acompositevariablethatincludes infor-mationaboutparents’income,levelofeducation,andthenumber ofchildren inthehousehold) usingtheChi-squaretest. Results demonstratedthatthenumberofparentsofboyswhoranked sci-enceasatopprioritywassignificantlyhigherthanthenumberof parentsofgirls(2(1,N=1085)=3.83,p=0.05,=0.06).Parentsof

olderchildrenweremorelikelytoranksciencewithintheir bot-tomthreechoices(2(2,N=1085)=8.52,p=0.014,=0.09).Also,

parentswithahigh-SESlevelweremorelikelytorankscienceasa toppriority(2(2,N=1085)=16.07,p<0.001,=0.12).

3.3. Two-stepclusteranalysis 3.3.1. Crossvalidation

Thedatasetwasrandomlysplitintotwohalveseachcontaining 728cases.Two-stepclusteranalyseswereperformedoneachhalf. Then,theviabilityofasimilarresultwasexaminedinthefull sam-ple.TheMann–WhitneyUtestindicatednosignificantdifferences betweenthesubsetsonclustervariables(ps>.01).

Clustermembershipinformation obtainedfromthetwo-step cluster analysis was used as a dependent variable in the dis-criminant analysis to obtain discriminant function coefficients associatedwiththeindependentvariables.Then,thesecoefficients wereusedtoclassifycasesintheseconddataset.Two-step clus-teranalysiswasperformedfortheseconddatasetandresulting clustermembershipinformationwascomparedtothe classifica-tioninformationcreatedbythediscriminantfunctioncoefficients. Resultsdemonstratedthat81%ofthecaseswerecorrectly classi-fied,whichishigherthanthepercentage(76%)suggestedbythe MaximumChanceCriterion.Two-stepclusteranalysesonthetwo subsetsandthefullsampleresultedinthesamenumberandhighly similarclustertypesemerging.Theseresultssuggestthatthe clus-tersolutionsacrossthesampleswerehighlystable.Therefore,the resultswerereportedforthefullsample.

3.3.2. Threeclusters

Thetwo-stepclusteranalysisidentifiedtwodistinctclustersof parentsplusanoutliercluster.Variableimportancevalues (ran-gingfrom0to1)demonstratedthatpre-writing(1.0),pre-reading (0.68),andmoralandethics(0.6)werethemostimportant vari-ablesin defining membershipstocluster 1and cluster2 while sciencewastheleastimportantvariable(0.13).Variable impor-tancevaluessuggestthatmostparentshadsimilarsciencerankings (low)andparentsdonotdeviateintheirprioritiesforscience.In otherwords,parents’sciencerankingswerenotusefulin determin-ingwhethertheyareamemberofthefirstorthesecondcluster.

Themajorityoftheparentswereinthefirstcluster.Thefirst clusterincludedparentswhotendtoprioritizetraditionalacademic contentareasincludingreading,writing,andmathematics.The sec-ondclusterincludedparentswhodecisivelyprefertheinclusionof moralsandethicsandartsintheearlychildhoodcurriculum.The lastclusterconsistedofparentswhoareidentifiedasoutliersby

thetwo-stepclusteranalysis.Parentsinthisclustertendtoprefer teachingofscienceandmathematicstotheiryoungsterintheearly childhoodclassrooms.Adetaileddescriptionoftheclusterprofiles isprovidedbelow.

3.3.3. Cluster1(ThreeR’s)

Thefirstclusterincluded736(50.5%)participants.Pre-reading, pre-writing,andmathematicswerehighlyratedbyparentsinthis cluster.Over90%oftheparentswhorankedpre-writingasthefirst (90.1%)orthesecond(94.1%)mostimportantacademiccontent areaswereinthiscluster.Therewereatotalof959participantsin thesamplewhorankedpre-writingwithintheirfirstthreechoices and727ofthem(76%)weremembersofthiscluster.Almost99% oftheclustermembersrankedpre-writingwithintheirfirstthree choices.

Over93%oftheparentswhorankedpre-readingasthefirst cur-ricularchoiceand74%oftheparentswhorankedpre-readingas thesecondmostimportantdomainwereinthiscluster.Therewere 966participantsinthedatasetwhorankedpre-readingwithintheir firstthreechoicesand728ofthem(75.4%)werethememberofthis cluster.Likethepre-writing,almost99%oftheclustermembers rankedpre-readingwithintheirfirstthreechoices.

Mathematicswasanotherimportantacademiccontentareafor themembersofthiscluster.Exactly39%oftheparentswhoranked mathematicsastheirfirstchoice,14.7%oftheparentswhoranked mathematicsastheirsecondchoice,and76.4%theparentswho rankedmathematicsastheirthirdchoicewereinthiscluster.There were 582participants in the sample who ranked mathematics withintheirfirstthreechoicesand318ofthem(54.6%)werethe memberofthiscluster.Over43%oftheclustermembersranked mathematicswithintheirfirstthreechoices.

Noneoftheparticipantsinthisclusterrankedsciencewithin theirfirsttwochoicesandonlysixparticipantsrankedscienceas thethirdmostimportanttopic.Therewere88participantsinthe samplewhorankedsciencewithintheirfirstthreechoicesandonly sixofthem(6.4%)werethemembersofthiscluster.Morethanhalf oftheparentsinthiscluster(473)rankedsciencewithintheirlast threechoices.

3.3.4. Cluster2(moralsandethicsandarts)

Thesecondclusterincluded415(28.5%)parents.Morals and ethicsandartswerehighlyratedacademiccontentareasbythe parentsinthiscluster.Almost81%oftheparentsinthiscluster rankedethicsastheirfirstchoiceand92.6%ofthecluster mem-bersrankedmoralsandethicswithintheirfirstthreechoices.There were500participantsinthesamplewhorankedethicsastheirfirst choiceand335ofthem(67%)wereinthiscluster.Likewise,there were804participantsinthesamplewhorankedethicswithintheir firstthreechoicesand384ofthem(48%)werethememberofthis cluster.

Morethanhalftheclustermembers(52.8%)rankedartswithin theirfirstthreechoices.Therewere392parentswhorankedarts withintheirfirst three choices inthe sampleand 219of them (55.9%)wereinthiscluster.Atotalof184parentsrankedartsas theirsecondchoiceinthesampleand149(81%)ofthemwerein thiscluster.Noneoftheparentsinthisclusterrankedscienceas theirfirstchoiceandonlyfiveparticipants(1.2%)rankedscienceas theirsecondorthirdmostimportantacademiccontentareas.The majorityoftheparents(92%)inthisclusterrankedsciencewithin theirlastthreechoices.

3.3.5. Cluster3(scienceandmathematics)

Therewere305(21%)parentsinthiscluster.Scienceand math-ematicsappearedtobethemostimportantacademiccontentareas fortheparentsinthiscluster.Atotalof88parentsrankedscience withintheirfirstthreechoicesinthesampleand77ofthem(87.5%)

(8)

wereinthiscluster.Inotherwords,theparentwhorankedscience asthefirstand97.1%whorankedthesecond,and80.8%whoranked thethirdmostimportantacademiccontentareawereinthis clus-ter.Aquarteroftheclustermembersrankedsciencewithintheir firstthreechoices.

Theresultsweresimilarforthedomainofmathematics.Over 55%oftheparentswhorankedmathematicsasthefirstand51.4% whorankeditasthesecondmostimportantareainthesample wereinthiscluster.Morethanhalfoftheparents(59.7%)inthis clusterrankedmathematicswithintheirfirstthreechoices.

Lessthanonefourthoftheparentsinthisclusterranked pre-readingandpre-writingwithintheirfirstthreechoices.Only17.1% oftheparentswhorankedmoralandethicswithintheirfirstthree choicesinthesamplewereinthiscluster.

3.4. Comparisonofclusterpriorities

Differencesinthepercentagesofpreferredcontentareasacross thethreeclusterswerecomparedusingtheChi-squaretest.The clusterswerecomparedbasedontheirmembers’prioritiesfor pre-reading,pre-writing,mathematics,science,moralandethics,and arts,whichemergedasmajoracademiccontentareasindefining clustermembership.Theresultsdemonstratedthatthenumberof parentswhorankedpre-readingasatopprioritywassignificantly higherinthefirstcluster(ThreeR’s)thanthenumberofparentsin thesecond(MoralsandEthicsandArts)(2(1,N=1151)=539.71,

p<0.01,=0.68)andthethirdcluster(ScienceandMathematics) (2(1,N=1041)=675.43,p<0.01,=0.81).Thenumberofparents

whoranked pre-readingasa topprioritywasalsosignificantly higherin thesecond clusterthanthenumberof parentsinthe thirdcluster(2(1,N=720)=17.14,p<0.01,=0.15).Likewise,the

numberofparentswhorankedpre-writingasatopprioritywas sig-nificantlyhigherinthefirstclusterthanthenumberofparentsin thesecond(2(1,N=1151)=507.24,p<0.01,=0.66)andthethird

cluster(2(1,N=1041)=735.64,p<0.01,=0.84).Thenumberof

parentswhorankedpre-writingasatopprioritywasalso signifi-cantlyhigherinthesecondclusterthanthenumberofparentsin thethirdcluster(2(1,N=720)=40.18,p<0.01,=0.24).

Ontheotherhand,thenumberofparentswhoranked mathe-maticsasatopprioritywassignificantlyhigherinthethirdcluster thanthenumber ofparents inthefirst (2(1,N=1041)=27.29,

p<0.01,=0.16)andthesecondcluster(2(1,N=720)=137.74,

p<0.01, =0.44). The number of parents who ranked mathe-maticsasatopprioritywasalsosignificantlyhigherinthefirst clusterthanthenumberof parentsinthesecondcluster(2(1,

N=1151)=70.07,p<0.01,=0.25).Likewise,thenumberof par-entswhorankedscienceasatopprioritywassignificantlyhigher in the third cluster than the number of parents in the first (2(1,N=1041)=175.41,p<0.01,=0.41)andthesecondcluster

(2(1,N=720)=100.68,p<0.01,=0.37).However,thedifference

betweenthefirstandthesecondclusterwasnotstatistically sig-nificant(2(1,N=1151)=0.43,p=0.51,=0.02).

Thenumberofparentswhorankedmoralsandethicsasatop prioritywassignificantlyhigherinthesecondclusterthanthe num-berofparentsinthefirst(2(1,N=1151)=318.48,p<0.01,=0.53)

and the third cluster (2(1, N=720)=199.26, p<0.01, =0.53).

However,thedifferencebetweenthefirstand thethirdcluster wasnot statisticallysignificant(2(1,N=1041)=3.75, p=0.053,

=0.06).Also,thenumberofparentswhorankedartsasatop prior-itywassignificantlyhigherinthesecondclusterthanthenumber ofparentsinthefirst(2(1,N=1151)=352.74,p<0.01,=0.55)

andthethirdcluster(2(1,N=720)=4.34,p=0.03,=0.08).The

numberofparentswhorankedartsasatopprioritywasalso sig-nificantlyhigherinthethirdclusterthanthenumberofparents inthefirstcluster(2(1,N=1041)=249.32,p<0.01,=0.49).

Col-lectively,theresultsofthechisquareanalysesalignwellwiththe

resultsoftheclusteranalysisandtheseresultssuggestthatclusters containhomogeneousgroupsofparentswithsimilarprioritiesfor academiccontentareasandparentalprioritiesdiffersignificantly acrossthethreeclusters.

3.5. Parents’socio-demographiccharacteristicsandcluster membership

Inordertoexploretherelationshipbetweencluster member-ship and parents’ socio-demographiccharacteristics, a seriesof analysisofvariance withBonferronicorrection(˛=0.0063) was conducted.Theresultsdemonstratedthatparentsinthreeclusters donotdifferintheirprioritiesbasedonthenumberofchildrenthey have(F(2,1453)=0.91,p=0.40),whethertheyhaveaboyoragirl (F(2,1453)=0.78,p=0.46),andwhethertheyaremotherorfather ofthechild(F(2,1453)=2.49,p=0.08).

However,therewasastatisticallysignificantdifferenceinthe followingsocio-demographiccharacteristicsbetweentheparents in threeclusters:parents’level ofeducation(F(2, 1453)=64.68, p<0.001, 2=0.01), child’s age (F(2, 1453)=54.32, p<0.001,

2=0.07),parents’income(F(2,1453)=48.65,p<0.001,2=0.06),

whetherparentshaveanolderchildwithpreviouspre-K experi-ences(F(2,1453)=5.57,p=0.004,2=0.01),andwhetherparents

resideoutsideoftheprovincialcapital(F(2,1453)=9.76,p<0.001, 2=0.01).

PosthoccomparisonsusingLSDtestsindicatedthatparentsin thefirstcluster(ThreeR’s)tendtohavealowereducationallevel andincome,haveanolderchild,andliveinruraldistricts(outsideof provincialcapital)thantheparentsinthesecond(EthicsandArts) andthethirdclusters(ScienceandMathematics)(ps<0.001).Also, significantlylessparentswithachildwithpreviouspre-K experi-enceswereinthesecondclustercomparedtothefirst(p<0.001) andthethirdcluster(p<0.043).

4. Discussion

Thefindingsofthepresentstudyindicatedthatthenumberof parentswhoprioritizescienceoverotheracademiccontentareas fortheirchildreninpre-Kclassroomsisquitelow.Whilethe major-ityoftheparents(68.4%) rankedsciencewithintheirlastthree choices,onlysixpercentoftheparentsrankedsciencewithintheir firstthreechoices.Scienceappearednottobeapopulartopicor ahighpriorityformanyparents,andfewparentspreferredtheir childrentolearnscienceintheearlychildhoodclassrooms.

Indeed,parentswhorankedsciencehighlydeviatedfromthe wholesampleandalmostalloftheseparentswereidentifiedas participantswithan unusualpreference pattern, which formed thescienceandmathematicscluster.Theparentsinthescience andmathematicsclusterwerealsodifferentfromtheparentsin theother clusterswithregard todemographic variables. These parentsweremorelikelytohaveahighereducationalleveland income, live in an urbandistrict, and have a younger child in comparison to the parents in the first cluster (Three R’s) and theyweremorelikelytohaveachildwithpreviouspre-K expe-riences than parents in the second cluster (Ethics and Arts). A comparison of parents who ranked sciencewithin theirfirst and last three choices also demonstrated similar findings. Par-ents’ofboysand youngerchildrenandparentswitha high-SES level were more likely to rank science within their first three choices.

Severalfactorsmightberesponsibleforthereasonmostparents rankedsciencelowerinthisstudy.Studiessuggestthatparental beliefsregardingtheirchildren’scompetencearelikelyto influ-ence the learning opportunities parents offer to their children (Dunton,McDevitt,&Hess,1988;Hess,Holloway,Dickson,&Price,

(9)

1984;Jacobs&Bleeker,2004;Miller,1995).Likemanyearly child-hoodteachers(Sac¸kes,inpress),mostparentsofpre-Kchildren mightbelievethattheirchildrenareincapableoflearningscience conceptsandskills,andtherefore,perceivelearningandteaching scienceinpre-Kclassroomsasadifficulttaskfortheirchildrenor theybelieveteachingsciencetoyoungchildrenisdevelopmentally inappropriate.

Parents’of boyswere more likely toinclude sciencewithin their first three choices than the parents’ of girls. This find-ingiscongruentwithpreviousstudiesongenderdifferences in parentalexpectationsregardingacademiccontentareas (Eccles, Jacobs, & Harold, 1990; Jacobs & Eccles, 1992). For example, parentsexpect boystoperformbetterin mathematicsand sci-encethangirls(Herbert&Stipek,2005;Tenenbaumetal.,2005). Thus,parentsaremorelikelytoofferlearningopportunitiesand providetoysandmaterialsthatsupportthedevelopmentof sci-entificandmathematicalthinkingskillsinboys(Alexanderetal., 2012;Freeman,2007;Simpkins,Davis-Kean,&Eccles,2005;Tracy, 1987). Even though there is no empirical evidence indicating that boys have an innate potential to perform better in sci-ence(Spelke,2005),boysareperceivedbytheirparentsasbeing more competent and eager to learn science(Andre, Whigham, Hendrickson,&Chambers,1999).Startingfromtheearly elemen-tary grades, boys tend to rate their science ability higher and reporthigher motivation tolearn sciencethan do girls (Andre etal.,1999;Breakwell,1992;Kahle&Lakes,1983;Patricketal., 2009;Simpkins,Davis-Kean,&Eccles,2006).Althoughstudieshave demonstratedthatthescienceachievementgaprelatedto gen-deris narrowing,theperceptionaland motivational differences betweenboysandgirlsregardingtheirlearningofscienceremains stable (Alexander et al., 2012; Baram-Tsabari& Yarden, 2010). Parentalbeliefsregardingtheimportanceandappropriatenessof learningsciencefortheirchildrenarelikelytoresultinboysand girlshavingdifferentialaccessand exposuretosciencelearning opportunitiesathomeandinearlychildhoodclassrooms.While thisearlysocializationmightsupportthedevelopmentof moti-vationalbeliefs thatfacilitatethelearning ofsciencein boys,it mightbedisadvantageousforgirls.Earlyparentalsupportmight explainwhythedifferenceinscienceachievementbetweengirls and boys becomes more prominent as they move into upper gradesand whyfemales are underrepresented inSTEM-related careers.

Inthepresentstudy,parentswithhigh-SESlevelsweremore likelytoratescienceasahigherprioritythantheparentswith low-SESlevels. Parents withhigh-SES appear tobe more sup-portiveoflearningscienceintheearlyyears.Thismightbedue tohigh-SESparentshavingnecessary economicalandcognitive resources to support children’s learning of science (Bradley & Corwyn,2002;Mantzicopoulosetal.,2013;Zady&Portes,2001). Alternatively,high-SESparentsmightbelievethattheirchildren arecompetentinbasicacademiccontentareastypicallytargeted inearlyyears,suchasliteracy.Hence,theyprefertheirchildren to excel in other content areas like science. Although parents withlow-SESvalueacademiclearningingeneral,theymightnot havetheresourcestosupporttheirchildren’slearningofscience (Zady&Portes,2001), thus,theydo not prioritizescience over otheracademic areas. Moreover,low-SESparents tend to have negativeexperiencesinlearningscienceandtheymightbe intim-idatedwhentheyneedtohelptheirchildrenin science-related learningtasks(Mantzicopoulosetal.,2013;Zady&Portes,2001). Parentsown experienceswithlearning sciencemight influence theirpreferencesforscienceeducationinpre-Kcurricula.Parents withapositiveattitudetowardlearningscienceandparentswho perceivescienceasfunmightbemorelikelytosupporttheir chil-dren’slearning of science(Dierking& Falk,1994; Ferry,Fouad, &Smith, 2000; George &Kaplan, 1998).High-SES parents and

parentswitha science-related educationandcareers mayhave positiveexperiencewithlearning scienceandperceivelearning scienceasa prerequisiteforsuccessinacademicandsociallife, whichmightexplainthehigh-SESparents’tendencytorate sci-enceasahigherprioritythanthelow-SESparentsinthecurrent study.

Children’s emergent scienceskills usuallyare not thetarget of instructional practices in typical earlychildhood classrooms (Sac¸kes, in press; Sac¸kes et al., 2011; Tu, 2006; Varol, 2013). Severalfactors,suchaslimitedscienceandpedagogicalcontent knowledge,thepressuretoteachothercontentareas,thelackof science-relatedmaterials,andlowself-efficacyforteachingscience havebeenidentifiedasreasonswhyteachersofyoungchildren devote less time toteaching science in theirclassrooms (Early etal.,2010;Garbett,2003;Greenfieldetal.,2009;Kallery&Psillos, 2001;Maieretal.,2013;Nayfeldetal.,2011).Thefindingsofthe present studysuggestthat parentalprioritiesmightbeanother majorfactorthatcontributesthelimitedvisibilityofscience learn-ingexperiencesintheearlyyears.Formorethantwo-thirdsofthe parents,sciencewastheleastpreferredacademic contentarea, whereasonlysixpercentoftheparentsinthisstudyprioritized providingsciencelearningexperiencestopre-Kchildren.Unlike literacy and mathematics, many parents do not expectscience to bea partof ordinary pre-K classrooms. The findings of the currentstudydemonstratedthatparentalpreferencesalignwell withtheearlychildhoodteachers’tendencytoteachlessscience (Greenfieldetal.,2009;Nayfeldetal.,2011;Sac¸kesetal.,2011; Varol,2013).

The present studyfocused onparental prioritiesfor science learning in pre-K classrooms by inviting parents to rank eight academiccontentareas.Futureinvestigationsshouldgatherdata about the rationale that guides parents’ rankings of academic contentareas.Morespecifically,futurestudiescouldinvestigate the reasons parents prefer (or do not prefer) that their chil-dren learn science during the earlyyears. In-depth interviews withtheparentswhoranked sciencewithintheirtop and bot-tom three choices regarding the reasons that motivate their preferences would help us better understand the factors that contributetothelimited visibilityofscienceinearlychildhood classrooms.Parentalprioritiesandpreferencesmightbe precur-sorstotheireducationalpracticesathome.Futurestudiesshould alsoinvestigatetherelationshipbetweenthequalityand quan-tityofinformalsciencelearningexperiencesprovidedbyparents andtheirprioritiesforscienceeducationinearlychildhood class-rooms.

Sofar,aconsiderableamountofliteraturehasbeenproduced onparentalprioritiesforearlychildhoodcurricula.However,the literaturetodate hastendedtofocus onparentalprioritiesfor developmentalareasandthetraditionalacademiccontentareas andskills(Dockett&Perry,2004;Ebbeck,1995;Kernan&Hayes, 1999;Knudsen-Lindauer&Harris,1989;Sac¸kes,2013).Parents’ beliefsaboutandtheirexpectationsandprioritiesforscience edu-cationintheearlyyearshavebeenlargelyneglected.Thecurrent studyprovidespreliminarydataonparentalviewsontheplace for science education within earlychildhood classrooms. More researchstudiesareneededtounderstandhowparental preferen-ces,beliefs,andattitudesinfluencesciencelearningopportunities providedtochildrenandchildren’slearningofscienceduringthe earlyyears.Parents’beliefsaboutandattitudestowardscienceare likelytoinfluencetheirchildren’sattitudetowardlearningscience andtheirmotivationtolaterpursuescience-relatedcareers(Chen, 2001;Dierking&Falk,1994;Ferryetal.,2000).Therefore, inves-tigatingparentalbeliefsaboutlearningscienceinearlychildhood classroomsmightbeakeytounderstandinggenderand socioeco-nomicdisparitiesinscienceachievementinlateelementarygrades andbeyond.

(10)

Acknowledgement

ThankstoBalıkesirProvincial Directorateof National Educa-tion,ResearchandDevelopmentUnitfortheirassistanceindata collection.

References

Aldenderfer,M.S.,&Blashfield,R.K.(1984).Clusteranalysis.NewburyParks,CA: Sage.

Alexander,J.M.,Johnson,K.E.,&Kelly,K.(2012).Longitudinalanalysisofthe relations betweenopportunities to learn about science and the develop-mentofinterestsrelatedtoscience.ScienceEducation,96(5),763–786,doi: 10.1002/sce.21018.

Anderson, R. D. (2002). Reforming science teaching: What research says about inquiry. Journal of Science Teacher Education, 13(1), 1–12, doi: 10.1023/A:1015171124982.

Anderson,R.D.(2007).Inquiryasanorganizingthemeforsciencecurricula.In S.Abell,&N.Lederman(Eds.),Handbookofresearchonscienceeducation(pp. 807–830).Mahwah,NJ:Erlbaum.

Andre, T., Whigham, M., Hendrickson, A., & Chambers, S. (1999). Compe-tencybeliefs,positiveaffect,andgenderstereotypesofelementarystudents and their parents about science versus other school subjects. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 36(6), 719–747, doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2736(199908)36:6<719::AID-TEA8>3.0.CO;2-R.

Appleton, K. (1992). Discipline knowledge and confidence to teach science: Self-perceptionsofprimaryteachereducationstudents.ResearchinScience Edu-cation,22(1),11–19,doi:10.1007/BF02356874.

Appleton,K.(1995).Studentteachers’confidencetoteachscience:Ismorescience knowledgenecessarytoimproveself-confidence.InternationalJournalofScience Education,17(3),357–369,doi:10.1080/0950069950170307.

Appleton,K.,&Kindt,I.(1999).Whyteachprimaryscience?Influencesonbeginning teachers’practices.InternationalJournalofScienceEducation,21(2),155–168, doi:10.1080/0950069950170307.

Aydın,A.,Sarıer,Y.,&Uysal,S¸.(2012).Sosyoekonomikvesosyokültürelde˘gis¸kenler ac¸ısından PISA matematik sonuc¸larının kars¸ılas¸tırılması [The comparative assessmentoftheresultsofPISAmathematicalliteracy intermsof socio-economicandsocio-culturalvariables].EducationandScience,37(164),20–30. Retrievedfrom.http://egitimvebilim.ted.org.tr/index.php/EB

Ayvacı,H.S¸.,Devecio˘glu,Y.,&Yi˘git,N.(2002).Okulöncesiö˘gretmenlerininfenve do˘gaetkinliklerindekiyeterliliklerininbelirlenmesi.In5.UlusalFenBilimlerive MatematikE˘gitimiKongresindesunulmus¸bildiri16–18Eylül,.Ankara,Türkiye: ODTÜ.

Balat, G. U., Beceren, B. Ö., & Özdemir, A. A. (2011). The evaluation of parents’ viewsrelated to helping pre-school children gain some univer-salvalues. Procedia- Social andBehavioral Sciences, 15(0), 908–912,doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2011.03.210.

Baldwin, J. L., Adams, S. M., & Kelly, M. K. (2009). Science at the center: Anemergent, standards-based,child-centered framework for early learn-ers.EarlyChildhoodEducationJournal,37,71–77,doi: 10.1007/s10643-009-0318-z.

Baram-Tsabari,A.,&Yarden,A.(2010).Quantifyingthegendergapinscience inter-ests.InternationalJournalofScienceandMathematicsEducation,9,523–550,doi: 10.1007/s10763-010-9194-7.

Bas¸türk,R.,&Is¸ıko˘glu,N.(2008).Analyzingprocessqualityofearlychildhood educationwithmanyfacetrashmeasurementmodel.EducationalSciences: TheoryandPractice,8(1),25–32.Retrievedfrom.http://edam.com.tr/kuyeb/ en/oncekisayilar.asp?act=detay&ID=33

Bekman, S. (2002). Does daycare centre experience and its aim matter? EuropeanEarly Childhood Education ResearchJournal, 10(1), 123–135,doi: 10.1080/13502930285208891.

Bekman, S. (2005). Early childhood education in Turkey: An overview. In O. Saracho, & B. Spodek (Eds.), International perspective on research in early childhood education (pp. 335–353). Charlotte, NC: Information Age.

Bekman, S.,&Gürlesel, C.F. (2005).(Theright start:Pre-primaryeducation in Turkey)Do˘grubas¸langıc¸:Türkiyedeokulöncesie˘gitim.Istanbul,Turkey:TÜS˙IAD [Turkish Industryand Business Association]. Retrieved from.http://www. tusiad.org.tr/ rsc/shared/file/egitim.pdf

Bekman,S.,&Göc¸er,A.(2005).EarlychildhoodeducationinTurkey.InC.Fisher, &R.Lerner(Eds.),Encyclopediaofapplieddevelopmentalscience(pp.380–384). ThousandOaks,CA:Sage.

Blake,E.,&Howitt,C.(2012).Scienceinearlylearningcentres:Satisfyingcuriosity, guidedplay,orlostopportunities.InK.C.D.Tam,M.S.Kim,&W.Hwang(Eds.), Movingforward:Issuesandchallengesinscienceeducationresearch(pp.281–299). Dordrecht,TheNetherlands:Springer.

Bodrova,E.,&Leong,D.J.(2003).Theimportanceofbeingplayful.Educational Lead-ership,60(7),50–53.

Bracken,S.S.,&Fischel,J.E.(2006).Assessmentofpreschoolclassroompractices: Applicationofq-sortmethodology.EarlyChildhoodResearchQuarterly,21(4), 417–430,doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2006.09.006.

Bradley, R. H., & Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 371–399, doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233.

Breakwell,G.M.(1992).Gender,parentalandpeerinfluencesuponscienceattitudes andactivities.PublicUnderstandingofScience,1(2),183–197,doi: 10.1088/0963-6625/1/2/003.

Brownlee,J.,Syu,J.-J.,Mascadri,J.,Cobb-Moore,C.,Walker,S.,Johansson,E.,etal. (2012).Teachers’andchildren’spersonalepistemologiesformoraleducation: Casestudiesinearlyyearselementaryeducation.TeachingandTeacher Educa-tion,28(3),440–450,doi:10.1016/j.tate.2011.11.012.

Callanan,M.A.,Jipson,J.L.,&Soennichsen,M.S.(2002).Maps,globes,andvideos: Parent-childconversationsaboutrepresentationalobjects.InS.G.Paris(Ed.), Perspectivesonobject-centeredlearninginmuseums(pp.261–283).Mahwah,NJ: Erlbaum.

Can-Yasar,M.,Inal,G.,Uyanik,O.,&Kandir,A.(2011).Ageneraloverviewofearly childhoodeducationandparentinvolvementactivitiesimplementedinTurkey. InternationalJournalofAcademicResearch,3(6),28–31.

Carey,S.(1985).Conceptualchangeinchildhood.Cambridge.MA:MITPress.

Chang,A.,Sandhofer,C.M.,&Brown,C.S.(2011).Genderbiasesinearlynumber exposuretopreschool-agedchildren.JournalofLanguageandSocialPsychology, 30(4),440–450,doi:10.1177/0261927X11416207.

Chen,H.(2001).Parents’attitudesandexpectationsregardingscienceeducation: ComparisonsamongAmerican,Chinese-America,andChinesefamilies. Adoles-cence,36(142),305–313.

Chiu,T.,Fang,D.,Chen,J.,Wang,Y.,&Jeris,C.(August,2001).Arobustand scal-ableclusteringalgorithmformixedtypeattributesinlargedatabaseenvironment. SanFrancisco,CA:PaperpresentedattheseventhACMSIGKDDinternational conferenceonKnowledgeDiscoveryandDataMining.

Crowley,K.,Callanan,M.A.,Tenenbaum,H.R.,&Allen,E.(2001).Parentsexplain moreoftentoboysthantogirlsduringsharedscientificthinking.Psychological Science,12(3),258–261,doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00347.

Cumming, J. (2003).Dorunner beansreallymake yourun fast?Young chil-dren learning about science-related food concepts in informal settings. ResearchinScienceEducation,33(4),483–502,doi:10.1023/B:RISE.0000005254. 53876.6e.

Derman, T. M., & Bas¸al, H. A. (2010). Cumhuriyetin ilanından günümüze Türkiye’de okul öncesi e˘gitim ve ilkö˘gretimde niceliksel ve niteliksel gelis¸meler [Qualitativeand quantitative developments and evaluationsin preschoolbetweenthefoundationofthe republicand today].Uluslararası Sosyal Aras¸tırmalar Dergisi, 3(11), 560–569. Retrieved from. http://www. sosyalarastirmalar.com/cilt3/sayi11pdf/tanermeralhandanbasal.pdf

Dierking,L.D.,&Falk,J.H.(1994).Familybehaviorandlearningininformal sci-encesettings: Areview oftheresearch.ScienceEducation,78,57–76,doi: 10.1002/sce.3730780104.

Dockett,S.,&Perry,B.(2004).Startingschool:PerspectivesofAustralianchildren, parentsandeducators.JournalofEarlyChildhoodResearch,2(2),171–189,doi: 10.1177/1476718X04042976.

Dunton,K.,McDevitt,T.,&Hess,R.(1988).Originsofmothers’attributionsabout theirdaughters’andsons’performanceinmathematicsinsixthgrade. Merrill-PalmerQuarterly,34(1),47–70.

Duschl,R.A.,Schweingruber,H.A.,&Shouse,A.(Eds.).(2007).Takingscienceto school:LearningandteachingscienceingradesK-8.Washington,DC:National AcademiesPress.

Early,D.M.,Iruka,I.U.,Ritchie,S.,Barbarin,O.A.,Winn,D.C.,Crawford,G.M...., etal.(2010).Howdopre-kindergartenersspendtheirtime?Gender,ethnicity andincomeaspredictorsofexperiencesinpre-kindergartenclassrooms.Early ChildhoodEducationQuarterly,25,177–193,doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2009.10.003.

Ebbeck,M.(1995).Purposesofearlychildhoodeducation:Expressedviewsof tea-chersandparentsinHongKong.InternationalJournalofEarlyYearsEducation, 3(2),3–18,doi:10.1080/0966976950030201.

Eccles,J.S.,Jacobs,J.E.,&Harold,R.D.(1990).Genderrolestereotypes,expectancy effects,andparents’socializationofgenderdifferences.JournalofSocialIssues, 46(2),183–201,doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.1990.tb01929.x.

Einarsdóttir,J. (2010).Icelandicparents’viewsonthenationalpolicyonearly childhoodeducation.EarlyYears:AnInternationalJournalofResearchand Devel-opment,30(3),229–242,doi:10.1080/09575146.2010.509059.

ErdillerAkın,Z.B.(2013).ExaminingthebeliefsofTurkishpreserviceearlychildhood teachersregardingearlychildhoodcurriculum.JournalofResearchinChildhood Education,27(3),302–318,doi:10.1080/02568543.2013.796331.

Erdiller,Z.B.,&McMullen,M.B.(2004).Turkishteachers’beliefsondevelopmentally appropriatepracticesinearlychildhoodeducation.HacettepeUniversityJournal ofEducation,25,84–93.Retrievedfrom.

Evans,P.,&Fuller,M.(1998).ThepurposesofnurseryeducationintheUK:Parent andchildperspectives.EuropeanEarlyChildhoodEducationResearchJournal,6(2), 35–53,doi:10.1080/13502939885208231.

Ferry, T. R., Fouad, N. A., & Smith, P. L. (2000). The role of family context in asocial cognitive modelfor acareer-related choice behavior:Amath and science perspective. Journal of VocationalBehavior, 57, 348–364,doi: 10.1006/jvbe.1999.1743.

Fleer,M.(2009).Supportingscientificconceptualconsciousnessorlearningin‘a roudaboutway’inplay-basedcontexts.InternationalJournalofScienceEducation, 31(8),1069–1089,doi:10.1080/09500690801953161.

Fleer,M.(2011).ConceptualPlay:Foregroundingimaginationandcognition dur-ingconceptformationinearlyyearseducation.ContemporaryIssuesinEarly Childhood,12(3),224–240,doi:10.2304/ciec.2011.12.3.224.

Freeman,N.K.(2007).Preschoolers’perceptionsofgenderappropriatetoysand theirparents’beliefsaboutgenderizedbehaviors:Miscommunication,mixed messages,orhiddentruths?EarlyChildhoodEducationJournal,34(5),357–366, doi:10.1007/s10643-006-0123-x.

Referanslar

Benzer Belgeler

Ev i ndcki banyoda sirtmi duvara yaslaml§ vc ayaklanm onc clogru uzatm l § oturur pozisyonda duran bir crkck ccscclinin tizcrinde bcyaz bir bluz, c;:ic;:ckli bir

In addition to this, the fact that the 25th World University Winter Games in 2011 and the European Youth Winter Festival in 2017 were in Erzurum has proved that the region has

Nevzat Kösoğlu’nun kaleme aldığı “Geçmiş Zaman Peşinde yahut Vaizin Söyledikleri” adlı anı kitabı da halk kültürü malzemesi barındırması bakımından

Çapanoğlu (2018) yaptığı çalışmada sağlık çalışanlarının çocuklarına aşı yaptırma veya yaptırmama ‘‘konusunda çocuğun üstün yararı’’ kavramı üzerinde çok

Pneumatosis cystoides intestinalis following steroid treatment in a nephrotic syndrome patient: report of a case.. The natural course of Clostridium perf- ringens-induced

Mais nous savons que pour saisir quelque chose en ce qui concerne notre langue maternelle, nous avons recours à un dictionnaire unilingue, alors que pour apprendre

The potential of digital mobile applications as learning tools will increase the availability of improved underlying technology, kits, and increased interaction

niyet müdrlüğünde görev alan Ahmet Samim, kısa bit zaman sonra Seday-ı Millet gazetesinin mesul müdürlüğü ile yazı işleri müdürlüğünü üzerine almış